Eye position compensation improves estimates of response magnitude and receptive field geometry in alert monkeys.

Studies of visual function in behaving subjects require that stimuli be positioned reliably on the retina in the presence of eye movements. Fixational eye movements scatter stimuli about the retina, inflating estimates of receptive field dimensions, reducing estimates of peak responses, and blurring maps of receptive field subregions. Scleral search coils are frequently used to measure eye position, but their utility for correcting the effects of fixational eye movements on receptive field maps has been questioned. Using eye coils sutured to the sclera and preamplifiers configured to minimize cable artifacts, we reexamined this issue in two rhesus monkeys. During repeated fixation trials, the eye position signal was used to adjust the stimulus position, compensating for eye movements and correcting the stimulus position to place it at the desired location on the retina. Estimates of response magnitudes and receptive field characteristics in V1 and in LGN were obtained in both compensated and uncompensated conditions. Receptive fields were narrower, with steeper borders, and response amplitudes were higher when eye movement compensation was used. In sum, compensating for eye movements facilitated more precise definition of the receptive field. We also monitored horizontal vergence over long sequences of fixation trials and found the variability to be low, as expected for this precise behavior. Our results imply that eye coil signals can be highly accurate and useful for optimizing visual physiology when rigorous precautions are observed.

[1]  H. Reitboeck,et al.  Fiber microelectrodes for electrophysiological recordings , 1983, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[2]  B. Richmond,et al.  Implantation of magnetic search coils for measurement of eye position: An improved method , 1980, Vision Research.

[3]  D. Snodderly,et al.  Spatial organization of receptive fields of V1 neurons of alert monkeys: comparison with responses to gratings. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  D H Hubel,et al.  Visual responses in V1 of freely viewing monkeys. , 1996, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[5]  Bruce G Cumming,et al.  Measuring V1 receptive fields despite eye movements in awake monkeys. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  D. Robinson,et al.  A METHOD OF MEASURING EYE MOVEMENT USING A SCLERAL SEARCH COIL IN A MAGNETIC FIELD. , 1963, IEEE transactions on bio-medical engineering.

[7]  M. Livingstone,et al.  Mechanisms of Direction Selectivity in Macaque V1 , 1998, Neuron.

[8]  D. Snodderly,et al.  Response Variability of Neurons in Primary Visual Cortex (V1) of Alert Monkeys , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  D. Fender,et al.  The interplay of drifts and flicks in binocular fixation. , 1969, Vision research.

[10]  D. Snodderly,et al.  Studying striate cortex neurons in behaving monkeys: Benefits of image stabilization , 1987, Vision Research.

[11]  S. Barash,et al.  Shift of visual fixation dependent on background illumination. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  Bevil R. Conway,et al.  Receptive Fields of Disparity-Tuned Simple Cells in Macaque V1 , 2003, Neuron.

[13]  B. Julesz,et al.  Extension of Panum's fusional area in binocularly stabilized vision. , 1967, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[14]  Doris Y. Tsao,et al.  Receptive fields of disparity-selective neurons in macaque striate cortex , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  D. Snodderly Effects of light and dark environments on macaque and human fixational eye movements , 1987, Vision Research.