The Representation of Three-Dimensional Space in Fish

In mammals, the so-called “seat of the cognitive map” is located in place cells within the hippocampus. Recent work suggests that the shape of place cell fields might be defined by the animals’ natural movement; in rats the fields appear to be laterally compressed (meaning that the spatial map of the animal is more highly resolved in the horizontal dimensions than in the vertical), whereas the place cell fields of bats are statistically spherical (which should result in a spatial map that is equally resolved in all three dimensions). It follows that navigational error should be equal in the horizontal and vertical dimensions in animals that travel freely through volumes, whereas in surface-bound animals would demonstrate greater vertical error. Here, we describe behavioral experiments on pelagic fish in which we investigated the way that fish encode three-dimensional space and we make inferences about the underlying processing. Our work suggests that fish, like mammals, have a higher order representation of space that assembles incoming sensory information into a neural unit that can be used to determine position and heading in three-dimensions. Further, our results are consistent with this representation being encoded isotropically, as would be expected for animals that move freely through volumes. Definitive evidence for spherical place fields in fish will not only reveal the neural correlates of space to be a deep seated vertebrate trait, but will also help address the questions of the degree to which environment spatial ecology has shaped cognitive processes and their underlying neural mechanisms.

[1]  K. Jeffery,et al.  Anisotropic encoding of three-dimensional space by place cells and grid cells , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[2]  J. Taube,et al.  Cue control and head direction cells. , 1998, Behavioral neuroscience.

[3]  R U Muller,et al.  Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. I. Description and quantitative analysis , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[4]  Robert I. Holbrook,et al.  Separate encoding of vertical and horizontal components of space during orientation in fish , 2009, Animal Behaviour.

[5]  Sensory System Affects Orientational Strategy in a Short‐Range Spatial Task in Blind and Eyed Morphs of the Fish, Astyanax fasciatus , 2009 .

[6]  T. Hafting,et al.  Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex , 2005, Nature.

[7]  Kate J. Jeffery,et al.  Neural encoding of large-scale three-dimensional space—properties and constraints , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[8]  Three-dimensional spatial learning in hummingbirds , 2013, Animal Behaviour.

[9]  Alex Kacelnik,et al.  Navigating in a volumetric world: metric encoding in the vertical axis of space. , 2013, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[10]  T. Guilford,et al.  Three-dimensional spatial cognition in a benthic fish, Corydoras aeneus , 2014, Behavioural Processes.

[11]  Nachum Ulanovsky,et al.  Neuroscience: How Is Three-Dimensional Space Encoded in the Brain? , 2011, Current Biology.

[12]  G. Taylor,et al.  Fractional rate of change of swim-bladder volume is reliably related to absolute depth during vertical displacements in teleost fish , 2010, Journal of the Royal Society Interface.

[13]  J. O'Keefe,et al.  The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. , 1971, Brain research.

[14]  Jeffrey Taube,et al.  Head direction cells , 2009, Scholarpedia.

[15]  Kathryn J Jeffery,et al.  Integration of the sensory inputs to place cells: What, where, why, and how? , 2007, Hippocampus.

[16]  James A. Ainge,et al.  Three-dimensional space: locomotory style explains memory differences in rats and hummingbirds , 2014, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[17]  Kate Jeffery,et al.  Horizontal biases in rats’ use of three-dimensional space , 2011, Behavioural Brain Research.

[18]  J. O’Keefe,et al.  Modeling place fields in terms of the cortical inputs to the hippocampus , 2000, Hippocampus.

[19]  Nachum Ulanovsky,et al.  Representation of Three-Dimensional Space in the Hippocampus of Flying Bats , 2013, Science.

[20]  Marie-Claude Grobéty,et al.  Spatial learning in a three-dimensional maze , 1992, Animal Behaviour.

[21]  Robert I. Holbrook,et al.  Fish navigation in the vertical dimension: can fish use hydrostatic pressure to determine depth? , 2011 .

[22]  Margaret F. Carr,et al.  Experience-Dependent Development of Coordinated Hippocampal Spatial Activity Representing the Similarity of Related Locations , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  F. Rodríguez,et al.  Spatial memory and hippocampal pallium through vertebrate evolution: insights from reptiles and teleost fish , 2002, Brain Research Bulletin.

[24]  R. Passingham The hippocampus as a cognitive map J. O'Keefe & L. Nadel, Oxford University Press, Oxford (1978). 570 pp., £25.00 , 1979, Neuroscience.

[25]  Robert I. Holbrook,et al.  Three-dimensional spatial cognition: freely swimming fish accurately learn and remember metric information in a volume , 2013, Animal Behaviour.

[26]  Susan D. Healy,et al.  Do rufous hummingbirds (Selasphorus rufus) use visual beacons? , 2010, Animal Cognition.

[27]  F. Rodríguez,et al.  Brain and Cognition in Teleost Fish , 2011 .

[28]  Robert I. Holbrook,et al.  Three-dimensional spatial representation in freely swimming fish , 2012, Cognitive Processing.

[29]  Robert I. Holbrook,et al.  Three-dimensional spatial cognition: information in the vertical dimension overrides information from the horizontal , 2011, Animal Cognition.