Cultural influences on neural basis of intergroup empathy

Cultures vary in the extent to which people prefer social hierarchical or egalitarian relations between individuals and groups. Here we examined the effect of cultural variation in preference for social hierarchy on the neural basis of intergroup empathy. Using cross-cultural neuroimaging, we measured neural responses while Korean and American participants observed scenes of racial ingroup and outgroup members in emotional pain. Compared to Caucasian-American participants, Korean participants reported experiencing greater empathy and elicited stronger activity in the left temporo-parietal junction (L-TPJ), a region previously associated with mental state inference, for ingroup compared to outgroup members. Furthermore, preferential reactivity within this region to the pain of ingroup relative to outgroup members was associated with greater preference for social hierarchy and ingroup biases in empathy. Together, these results suggest that cultural variation in preference for social hierarchy leads to cultural variation in ingroup-preferences in empathy, due to increased engagement of brain regions associated with representing and inferring the mental states of others.

[1]  Ralph Weidner,et al.  What is “Odd” in Posner's Location-cueing Paradigm? Neural Responses to Unexpected Location and Feature Changes Compared , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[2]  C. Lumsden Culture and the Evolutionary Process, Robert Boyd, Peter J. Richerson. University of Chicago Press, Chicago & London (1985), viii, +301. Price $29.95 , 1986 .

[3]  M. Brewer The Psychology of Prejudice: Ingroup Love and Outgroup Hate? , 1999 .

[4]  J. Decety,et al.  The Role of the Right Temporoparietal Junction in Social Interaction: How Low-Level Computational Processes Contribute to Meta-Cognition , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[5]  F. Pratto,et al.  In-Group Identification, Social Dominance Orientation, and Differential Intergroup Social Allocation , 1994 .

[6]  R. Nisbett,et al.  Culture and systems of thought: holistic versus analytic cognition. , 2001, Psychological review.

[7]  K. Preuschoff,et al.  Neural Responses to Ingroup and Outgroup Members' Suffering Predict Individual Differences in Costly Helping , 2010, Neuron.

[8]  Xiaoying Wang,et al.  Do You Feel My Pain? Racial Group Membership Modulates Empathic Neural Responses , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  G. Hofstede The Cultural Relativity of Organizational Practices and Theories , 1983 .

[10]  M. Raichle,et al.  Anatomic Localization and Quantitative Analysis of Gradient Refocused Echo-Planar fMRI Susceptibility Artifacts , 1997, NeuroImage.

[11]  Natasa Kovacevic,et al.  Patterns of Autobiographical Memory Loss in Medial-Temporal Lobe Amnesic Patients , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[12]  F. Pratto,et al.  Social Dominance: An Intergroup Theory of Social Hierarchy and Oppression , 1999 .

[13]  J. Talairach,et al.  Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging , 1988 .

[15]  Tokiko Harada,et al.  Theory and methods in cultural neuroscience. , 2010, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[16]  Tokiko Harada,et al.  Neural Basis of Preference for Human Social Hierarchy versus Egalitarianism , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[17]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Attentional systems in target and distractor processing: a combined ERP and fMRI study , 2004, NeuroImage.

[18]  Mark H. Davis Empathy: A Social Psychological Approach , 1994 .

[19]  B. Malle,et al.  Social dominance orientation: A personality variable predicting social and political attitudes. , 1994 .

[20]  J. Dovidio,et al.  When and How Do High Status Group Members Offer Help: Effects of Social Dominance Orientation and Status Threat , 2008 .

[21]  M. Inzlicht,et al.  Empathy constrained: Prejudice predicts reduced mental simulation of actions during observation of outgroups. , 2010 .

[22]  L. Caporael The Evolution of Truly Social Cognition: The Core Configurations Model , 1997, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[23]  Joan Y. Chiao,et al.  Cultural neuroscience: a once and future discipline. , 2009, Progress in brain research.

[24]  Joseph C. Carroll Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology , 2008 .

[25]  S. Kitayama,et al.  Handbook of Cultural Psychology , 2007 .

[26]  C. Neil Macrae,et al.  Forming impressions of people versus inanimate objects: Social-cognitive processing in the medial prefrontal cortex , 2005, NeuroImage.

[27]  Joan Y. Chiao,et al.  Intergroup Empathy: How Does Race Affect Empathic Neural Responses? , 2010, Current Biology.

[28]  Douglas T. Kenrick,et al.  Human evolution and social cognition , 2007 .

[29]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[30]  G. Northoff,et al.  Culture-sensitive neural substrates of human cognition: a transcultural neuroimaging approach , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[31]  Dana Samson,et al.  Left temporoparietal junction is necessary for representing someone else's belief , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  Michael A. Becker Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles , 1998 .

[33]  R. Nathan Spreng,et al.  Patterns of Brain Activity Supporting Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, and Theory of Mind, and Their Relationship to the Default Mode Network , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  Tania Singer,et al.  University of Zurich Zurich Open Repository and Archive I Feel How You Feel but Not Always: the Empathic Brain and Its I Feel How You Feel but Not Always: the Empathic Brain and Its Modulation , 2009 .

[35]  Rebecca Elliott,et al.  Neuronal correlates of theory of mind and empathy: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study in a nonverbal task , 2006, NeuroImage.

[36]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Us Versus Them , 2011, Psychological science.

[37]  Moshe Bar,et al.  Cultural Specificity in Amygdala Response to Fear Faces , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[38]  L. Caporael,et al.  1. Evolutionary theory for social and cultural psychology , 2007 .

[39]  Gary H. Glover,et al.  Reducing interscanner variability of activation in a multicenter fMRI study: Controlling for signal-to-fluctuation-noise-ratio (SFNR) differences , 2006, NeuroImage.

[40]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[41]  R. Nisbett,et al.  Running Head: CULTURE AND THOUGHT Culture and Systems of Thought: Holistic vs. Analytic Cognition , 2001 .

[42]  Joan Y. Chiao,et al.  Cultural neuroscience: Parsing universality and diversity across levels of analysis. , 2007 .

[43]  A. Sirigu,et al.  Racial Bias Reduces Empathic Sensorimotor Resonance with Other-Race Pain , 2010, Current Biology.

[44]  Joan Y. Chiao,et al.  Culture–gene coevolution of individualism–collectivism and the serotonin transporter gene , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[45]  Marilynn B. Brewer,et al.  Taking the Social Origins of Human Nature Seriously: Toward a More Imperialist Social Psychology , 2004, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[46]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  Overlapping and non-overlapping brain regions for theory of mind and self reflection in individual subjects. , 2006, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[47]  M. Schaller,et al.  Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology , 2007 .

[48]  R. Brislin Back-Translation for Cross-Cultural Research , 1970 .

[49]  R. Saxe,et al.  What gets the attention of the temporo-parietal junction? An fMRI investigation of attention and theory of mind , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[50]  Uta Frith,et al.  Theory of mind , 2001, Current Biology.

[51]  Tokiko Harada,et al.  Neural basis of extraordinary empathy and altruistic motivation , 2010, NeuroImage.

[52]  G. Hofstede,et al.  Culture′s Consequences: International Differences in Work-Related Values , 1980 .

[53]  J. Decety,et al.  What you believe versus what you think they believe: a neuroimaging study of conceptual perspective‐taking , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[54]  R. Buckner,et al.  Self-projection and the brain , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[55]  Lindsey J. Powell,et al.  It's the Thought That Counts , 2006, Psychological science.

[56]  R. Saxe,et al.  Making sense of another mind: The role of the right temporo-parietal junction , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[57]  Damian R. Murray,et al.  Pathogen prevalence predicts human cross-cultural variability in individualism/collectivism , 2008, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[58]  E. Higgins,et al.  Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles. , 1996 .

[59]  R. Nathan Spreng,et al.  The Common Neural Basis of Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, Navigation, Theory of Mind, and the Default Mode: A Quantitative Meta-analysis , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[60]  Joe C Magee,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Power and Perspectives Not Taken , 2022 .

[61]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[62]  Ying-yi Hong,et al.  Dynamic Influences of Culture on Cooperation in the Prisoner's Dilemma , 2005, Psychological science.

[63]  J. Decety,et al.  A Social-Neuroscience Perspective on Empathy , 2006 .

[64]  Donald T. Stuss,et al.  Common and Unique Neural Correlates of Autobiographical Memory and Theory of Mind , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.