My Brain : Self and other representations in social cognitive neuroscience

Self and other representations 2 " Say this blanket represents all the matter and energy in the universe, okay? This is me, this is you. And over here, this is the Eiffel Tower, right, it's Paris! "-Bernard Jaffe, I Heart Huckabees In the film, " I Heart Huckabees " the magic of digital effects allows characters to see bits of themselves appear in the pixilated faces of others. Soon after experiencing this literal mirroring of him or herself in someone else, each character is moved to act compassionately, even towards previous enemies. Vaguely echoing Eastern philosophy, one of the main characters (as quoted above) claims that in moments of clarity, people come to understand that they are actually made from the same blanket, so to speak, and that self/other distinctions are an illusion. Does dissolving boundaries between ourselves and others actually help us to navigate the social world? Do we, in fact, understand the mental and emotional states of others using processes that are similar to those we use to think about ourselves? As with most psychological questions this broad, the answer is most likely both yes and no. On the one hand, behavioral research suggests that quite often we use ourselves as a template or anchor when trying to piece together the contents of someone else's mind (Epley, Keysar, Van Boven, & Gilovich, 2004). This overlap is attested to by our astonishing success at quickly inferring and learning from the goals of others, a type of learning that would only be possible if we understood others as operating much like we do, in pursuit of goals much like our own (Tomasello, 2000). These tendencies produce predictable errors as well. For example, before their 4 th birthday, the majority of children despotically assume that others see the world the way they do, and it takes the development of inhibitory control to quell this tendency and enable children to understand that others have thoughts and desires independent from their own (Carlson & Moses, 2001). Self and other representations 3 Similarly, adults will incorrectly guess that things they have just learned (e.g. the brand names of two sodas in a taste test) will be known by others who have not had the benefit of having the answers told to them, and it takes cognitive effort to override this assumed overlap and correctly judge other people's state of knowledge On the other hand, …

[1]  R. J Dolan,et al.  Activity in ventromedial prefrontal cortex covaries with sympathetic skin conductance level: a physiological account of a “default mode” of brain function , 2004, NeuroImage.

[2]  Richard J. Maddock,et al.  Separating subjective emotion from the perception of emotion-inducing stimuli: An fMRI study , 2006, NeuroImage.

[3]  K. Scherer,et al.  Appraisal processes in emotion. , 2003 .

[4]  R. Saxe,et al.  Making sense of another mind: The role of the right temporo-parietal junction , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  Kevin L. Harrell,et al.  Empathic joy and the empathy-altruism hypothesis. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[6]  Jane F. Banfield,et al.  Medial prefrontal activity predicts memory for self. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[7]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Principles, processes, and puzzles of social cognition: An introduction for the special issue on social cognitive neuroscience , 2005, NeuroImage.

[8]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[9]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Rethinking Feelings: An fMRI Study of the Cognitive Regulation of Emotion , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[10]  G. Shulman,et al.  Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[11]  Simon J Graham,et al.  An fMRI study investigating cognitive modulation of brain regions associated with emotional processing of visual stimuli , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  N. Epley,et al.  Perspective taking as egocentric anchoring and adjustment. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[13]  Kevin A. Pelphrey,et al.  Grasping the Intentions of Others: The Perceived Intentionality of an Action Influences Activity in the Superior Temporal Sulcus during Social Perception , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  Mark H. Davis,et al.  Effect of perspective taking on the cognitive representation of persons: a merging of self and other. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[15]  C. Geula,et al.  Cytoarchitecture and neural afferents of orbitofrontal cortex in the brain of the monkey , 1992, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[16]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[17]  M. Mesulam,et al.  Insula of the old world monkey. III: Efferent cortical output and comments on function , 1982, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[18]  N. Alpert,et al.  Conscious recollection and the human hippocampal formation: evidence from positron emission tomography. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[19]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: A relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[20]  Jeffrey S. Maxwell,et al.  Human Amygdala Responsivity to Masked Fearful Eye Whites , 2004, Science.

[21]  Karen Gasper,et al.  Attending to the Big Picture: Mood and Global Versus Local Processing of Visual Information , 2002, Psychological science.

[22]  R Bandler,et al.  Orbitomedial prefrontal cortical projections to hypothalamus in the rat , 2001, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[23]  E. Harmon-Jones,et al.  Empathy and attitudes: can feeling for a member of a stigmatized group improve feelings toward the group? , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[24]  J. Gross,et al.  The cognitive control of emotion , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  Steven L. Neuberg,et al.  Reinterpreting the empathy-altruism relationship: when one into one equals oneness. , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[26]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Empathy examined through the neural mechanisms involved in imagining how I feel versus how you feel pain , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  M. Brass,et al.  Imitation: is cognitive neuroscience solving the correspondence problem? , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  Robert T. Knight,et al.  The impact of orbital prefrontal cortex damage on emotional activation to unanticipated and anticipated acoustic startle stimuli , 2004, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[29]  S. Rauch,et al.  A functional MRI study of human amygdala responses to facial expressions of fear versus anger. , 2001, Emotion.

[30]  Steven Laureys,et al.  Cytology and functionally correlated circuits of human posterior cingulate areas , 2006, NeuroImage.

[31]  David A. Lishner,et al.  Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin Moral Action? "... as You Would Have Them Do Unto You": Does Imagining Yourself in the Other's Place Stimulate , 2022 .

[32]  C. Frith,et al.  Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  A unifying view of the basis of social cognition , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[34]  G. Schoenbaum,et al.  Neural Encoding in Orbitofrontal Cortex and Basolateral Amygdala during Olfactory Discrimination Learning , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  K. Ochsner,et al.  Social Cognitive Neuroscience 1 Social Cognitive Neuroscience: Historical Development, Core Principles, and Future Promise , 2005 .

[36]  Jamil Zaki,et al.  Your pain or mine? Common and distinct neural systems supporting the perception of pain in self and other. , 2008, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[37]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Dual-process theories in social psychology , 1999 .

[38]  J. Gross,et al.  Thinking makes it so: A social cognitive neuroscience approach to emotion regulation. , 2004 .

[39]  R. Saxe Against simulation: the argument from error , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[40]  J. Lanzetta,et al.  Vicarious instigation and conditioning of facial expressive and autonomic responses to a model's expressive display of pain. , 1980, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[41]  M. Hallett,et al.  Modeling other minds , 1995, Neuroreport.

[42]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. , 1999, Science.

[43]  Georg Northoff,et al.  Self-referential processing in our brain—A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self , 2006, NeuroImage.

[44]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Neural processing of emotional faces requires attention , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[45]  Robert T. Knight,et al.  Selective impairment of reasoning about social exchange in a patient with bilateral limbic system damage , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[46]  Andrew N. Meltzoff,et al.  How do we perceive the pain of others? A window into the neural processes involved in empathy , 2005, NeuroImage.

[47]  E. Kandel,et al.  Resolving Emotional Conflict: A Role for the Rostral Anterior Cingulate Cortex in Modulating Activity in the Amygdala , 2006, Neuron.

[48]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension , 1995, Cognition.

[49]  J. Decety,et al.  Effect of subjective perspective taking during simulation of action: a PET investigation of agency , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[50]  A. Cavanna,et al.  The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[51]  C. N. Macrae,et al.  Finding the Self? An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[52]  A. Damasio,et al.  Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[53]  G. de Marco,et al.  Changes in effective connectivity during incidental and intentional perception of fearful faces , 2006, NeuroImage.

[54]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Visual awareness and the detection of fearful faces. , 2005, Emotion.

[55]  Hugo D. Critchley,et al.  Brain activity relating to the contingent negative variation: an fMRI investigation , 2004, NeuroImage.

[56]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[57]  L. Fogassi,et al.  A Touching Sight SII/PV Activation during the Observation and Experience of Touch , 2004, Neuron.

[58]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others , 2006, Nature.

[59]  S. Rauch,et al.  Masked Presentations of Emotional Facial Expressions Modulate Amygdala Activity without Explicit Knowledge , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[60]  F. Craik,et al.  In search of the emotional self: an fMRI study using positive and negative emotional words. , 2003, The American journal of psychiatry.

[61]  G. Pellegrino,et al.  Vicarious responses to pain in anterior cingulate cortex: Is empathy a multisensory issue? , 2004, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[62]  Mark Lau,et al.  Modulation of cortical-limbic pathways in major depression: treatment-specific effects of cognitive behavior therapy. , 2004, Archives of general psychiatry.

[63]  E. Rolls The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex , 1999, Brain and Cognition.

[64]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Both of Us Disgusted in My Insula The Common Neural Basis of Seeing and Feeling Disgust , 2003, Neuron.

[65]  C. Batson,et al.  Five studies testing two new egoistic alternatives to the empathy-altruism hypothesis. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[66]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Distinct neural systems subserve person and object knowledge , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[67]  Kevin N. Ochsner,et al.  For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion , 2004, NeuroImage.

[68]  Jennifer S. Beer,et al.  The regulatory function of self-conscious emotion: insights from patients with orbitofrontal damage. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[69]  B. Mesquita,et al.  The experience of emotion. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.

[70]  H. Mayberg Limbic-cortical dysregulation: a proposed model of depression. , 1997, The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[71]  A. Raine,et al.  Reduced prefrontal gray matter volume and reduced autonomic activity in antisocial personality disorder. , 2000, Archives of general psychiatry.

[72]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  The motor theory of social cognition: a critique , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[73]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Dissociable Medial Prefrontal Contributions to Judgments of Similar and Dissimilar Others , 2006, Neuron.

[74]  C. Curtis,et al.  Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[75]  D. Perrett,et al.  A region of right posterior superior temporal sulcus responds to observed intentional actions , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[76]  G. Fink,et al.  Neural correlates of the first-person-perspective , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[77]  T. Wager,et al.  Different circuits for different pain: Patterns of functional connectivity reveal distinct networks for processing pain in self and others , 2007, Social neuroscience.

[78]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Empathy for Pain Involves the Affective but not Sensory Components of Pain , 2004, Science.

[79]  Jeroen Vaes,et al.  The Emotional Side of Prejudice: The Attribution of Secondary Emotions to Ingroups and Outgroups , 2000 .

[80]  H. Sackeim,et al.  Parietal cortex and representation of the mental Self. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[81]  N. Eisenberg,et al.  The relation of empathy to prosocial and related behaviors. , 1987, Psychological bulletin.

[82]  Kevin N. Ochsner,et al.  The neural correlates of direct and reflected self-knowledge , 2005, NeuroImage.

[83]  Riitta Hari,et al.  Activation of the human primary motor cortex during observation of tool use , 2004, NeuroImage.

[84]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  The emergence of social cognitive neuroscience. , 2001, The American psychologist.

[85]  L. Moses,et al.  Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind. , 2001, Child development.

[86]  Scott T Grafton,et al.  Functional imaging of face and hand imitation: towards a motor theory of empathy , 2004, NeuroImage.

[87]  J. Bargh,et al.  The automated will: nonconscious activation and pursuit of behavioral goals. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[88]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Social cognitive neuroscience: a review of core processes. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.

[89]  Amishi P. Jha,et al.  Viewing facial expressions of pain engages cortical areas involved in the direct experience of pain , 2005, NeuroImage.

[90]  M. Beauregard,et al.  Neural Correlates of Conscious Self-Regulation of Emotion , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[91]  K. Luan Phan,et al.  Functional Neuroanatomy of Emotion: A Meta-Analysis of Emotion Activation Studies in PET and fMRI , 2002, NeuroImage.

[92]  A. L. Beaman,et al.  Empathy-based helping: is it selflessly or selfishly motivated? , 1987, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[93]  Mark Snyder,et al.  Prosocial emotions and helping: the moderating role of group membership. , 2005, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[94]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Psychophysiological and Modulatory Interactions in Neuroimaging , 1997, NeuroImage.

[95]  C. Neil Macrae,et al.  Forming impressions of people versus inanimate objects: Social-cognitive processing in the medial prefrontal cortex , 2005, NeuroImage.

[96]  T. Allison,et al.  Functional anatomy of biological motion perception in posterior temporal cortex: an FMRI study of eye, mouth and hand movements. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[97]  M. Corbetta,et al.  An Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of Voluntary and Stimulus-Driven Orienting of Attention , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[98]  M. Iacoboni Neural mechanisms of imitation , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[99]  B. Englis,et al.  Expectations of Cooperation and Competition and Their Effects on Observers' Vicarious Emotional Responses , 1989 .

[100]  Andreas Olsson,et al.  Learned Fear of “Unseen” Faces after Pavlovian, Observational, and Instructed Fear , 2004, Psychological science.

[101]  C. Frith,et al.  Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[102]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  In search of occipital activation during visual mental imagery , 1994, Trends in Neurosciences.

[103]  C. Neil Macrae,et al.  The Link between Social Cognition and Self-referential Thought in the Medial Prefrontal Cortex , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[104]  Raymond J. Dolan,et al.  Levels of appraisal: A medial prefrontal role in high-level appraisal of emotional material , 2006, NeuroImage.