Large-Scale Brain Networks in Board Game Experts: Insights from a Domain-Related Task and Task-Free Resting State

Cognitive performance relies on the coordination of large-scale networks of brain regions that are not only temporally correlated during different tasks, but also networks that show highly correlated spontaneous activity during a task-free state. Both task-related and task-free network activity has been associated with individual differences in cognitive performance. Therefore, we aimed to examine the influence of cognitive expertise on four networks associated with cognitive task performance: the default mode network (DMN) and three other cognitive networks (central-executive network, dorsal attention network, and salience network). During fMRI scanning, fifteen grandmaster and master level Chinese chess players (GM/M) and fifteen novice players carried out a Chinese chess task and a task-free resting state. Modulations of network activity during task were assessed, as well as resting-state functional connectivity of those networks. Relative to novices, GM/Ms showed a broader task-induced deactivation of DMN in the chess problem-solving task, and intrinsic functional connectivity of DMN was increased with a connectivity pattern associated with the caudate nucleus in GM/Ms. The three other cognitive networks did not exhibit any difference in task-evoked activation or intrinsic functional connectivity between the two groups. These findings demonstrate the effect of long-term learning and practice in cognitive expertise on large-scale brain networks, suggesting the important role of DMN deactivation in expert performance and enhanced functional integration of spontaneous activity within widely distributed DMN-caudate circuitry, which might better support high-level cognitive control of behavior.

[1]  P. Fransson Spontaneous low‐frequency BOLD signal fluctuations: An fMRI investigation of the resting‐state default mode of brain function hypothesis , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[2]  N. Charness Visual short-term memory and aging in chess players. , 1981, Journal of gerontology.

[3]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[4]  Paul J. Feltovich,et al.  The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance , 2006 .

[5]  B. Biswal,et al.  Functional connectivity of human striatum: a resting state FMRI study. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[6]  Pietro Pietrini,et al.  Brain activity in chess playing , 1994, Nature.

[7]  S. Bressler,et al.  Large-scale brain networks in cognition: emerging methods and principles , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[8]  Jutta S. Mayer,et al.  Specialization in the default mode: Task‐induced brain deactivations dissociate between visual working memory and attention , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[9]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  The Neural Basis of Intuitive Best Next-Move Generation in Board Game Experts , 2011, Science.

[10]  Abraham Z. Snyder,et al.  A method for using blocked and event-related fMRI data to study “resting state” functional connectivity , 2007, NeuroImage.

[11]  John M Henderson,et al.  The Role of Meaning in Contextual Cueing: Evidence from Chess Expertise , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[12]  M. Raichle,et al.  The neural correlates of consciousness: an analysis of cognitive skill learning. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[13]  John A. E. Anderson,et al.  A multivariate analysis of age-related differences in default mode and task-positive networks across multiple cognitive domains. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[14]  S. Petersen,et al.  The effects of practice on the functional anatomy of task performance. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[15]  F. Gobet,et al.  The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance , 2006 .

[16]  N. Charness The impact of chess research on cognitive science , 1992 .

[17]  Fernand Gobet,et al.  Structure and Stimulus Familiarity: A Study of Memory in Chess-Players with Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2005, The Spanish Journal of Psychology.

[18]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Learning sculpts the spontaneous activity of the resting human brain , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[19]  A. Grinvald,et al.  Linking spontaneous activity of single cortical neurons and the underlying functional architecture. , 1999, Science.

[20]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Intrinsic Fluctuations within Cortical Systems Account for Intertrial Variability in Human Behavior , 2007, Neuron.

[21]  A. Sanfey Social Decision-Making : Insights from Game Theory and Neuroscience , 2022 .

[22]  R. Sperling,et al.  Age-related memory impairment associated with loss of parietal deactivation but preserved hippocampal activation , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[23]  T Elbert,et al.  Pattern of focal gamma-bursts in chess players. , 2001, Nature.

[24]  M. Raichle The Brain's Dark Energy , 2006, Science.

[25]  Habib Benali,et al.  Identification of large-scale networks in the brain using fMRI , 2006, NeuroImage.

[26]  Yong-Ju Kwon,et al.  Changes in brain activation induced by the training of hypothesis generation skills: An fMRI study , 2009, Brain and Cognition.

[27]  B. Balleine,et al.  Reward‐guided learning beyond dopamine in the nucleus accumbens: the integrative functions of cortico‐basal ganglia networks , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.

[28]  M. Raichle,et al.  Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[29]  M. Greicius,et al.  Default-mode network activity distinguishes Alzheimer's disease from healthy aging: Evidence from functional MRI , 2004, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA.

[30]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Analysis of fMRI Time-Series Revisited , 1995, NeuroImage.

[31]  P. Skudlarski,et al.  Brain Connectivity Related to Working Memory Performance , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  Michelle Hampson,et al.  Connectivity–behavior analysis reveals that functional connectivity between left BA39 and Broca's area varies with reading ability , 2006, NeuroImage.

[33]  C. Summerfield,et al.  An information theoretical approach to prefrontal executive function , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[34]  S. Quartz,et al.  Getting to Know You: Reputation and Trust in a Two-Person Economic Exchange , 2005, Science.

[35]  Hillary S. Schaefer,et al.  Neural correlates of attentional expertise in long-term meditation practitioners , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[36]  A. Graybiel Habits, rituals, and the evaluative brain. , 2008, Annual review of neuroscience.

[37]  Fernand Gobet,et al.  Adaptive Expert Decision Making: Skilled Chess Players Search More and Deeper , 2004, J. Int. Comput. Games Assoc..

[38]  A. Graybiel The basal ganglia: learning new tricks and loving it , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[39]  Vince D. Calhoun,et al.  Alterations in Memory Networks in Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer's Disease: An Independent Component Analysis , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  Sheng He,et al.  A functional MRI study of high-level cognition , 2003 .

[41]  F. Gobet The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance , 2006 .

[42]  M. Onofrj,et al.  Non-dominant dorsal-prefrontal activation during chess problem solution evidenced by single photon emission computerized tomography (SPECT) , 1995, Neuroscience Letters.

[43]  Satoshi Mochizuki,et al.  Learning-related changes of brain activation in the visual ventral stream: An fMRI study of mirror reading skill , 2006, Brain Research.

[44]  Yufeng Zang,et al.  DPARSF: A MATLAB Toolbox for “Pipeline” Data Analysis of Resting-State fMRI , 2010 .

[45]  Wei Zhang,et al.  Selective aberrant functional connectivity of resting state networks in social anxiety disorder , 2010, NeuroImage.

[46]  R. Buckner,et al.  Self-projection and the brain , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[47]  A. Binet Mnemonic virtuosity: a study of chess players. , 1966, Genetic psychology monographs.

[48]  B. Balleine,et al.  The integrative function of the basal ganglia in instrumental conditioning , 2009, Behavioural Brain Research.

[49]  R. Turner,et al.  Characterizing Evoked Hemodynamics with fMRI , 1995, NeuroImage.

[50]  L. Barrett,et al.  Do chess and GO need ‘g’? , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[51]  Vinod Menon,et al.  Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[52]  H. Simon,et al.  Perception in chess , 1973 .

[53]  B. Biswal,et al.  Functional connectivity of default mode network components: Correlation, anticorrelation, and causality , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[54]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[55]  M. Petrides,et al.  Functional role of the basal ganglia in the planning and execution of actions , 2006, Annals of neurology.

[56]  K. Davis,et al.  Cognitive and default‐mode resting state networks: Do male and female brains “rest” differently? , 2010, Human brain mapping.

[57]  R. Nathan Spreng,et al.  The Common Neural Basis of Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, Navigation, Theory of Mind, and the Default Mode: A Quantitative Meta-analysis , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[58]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[59]  Herbert A. Simon,et al.  The game of chess , 1992 .

[60]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Independent component model of the default-mode brain function: Assessing the impact of active thinking , 2006, Brain Research Bulletin.

[61]  J. Binder,et al.  A Parametric Manipulation of Factors Affecting Task-induced Deactivation in Functional Neuroimaging , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[62]  B. Harrison,et al.  Task-Induced Deactivation from Rest Extends beyond the Default Mode Brain Network , 2011, PloS one.

[63]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[64]  J. Desmond,et al.  The neural basis of visual skill learning: an fMRI study of mirror reading. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[65]  Neil Charness,et al.  Expertise in chess , 2006 .

[66]  G. Glover,et al.  Dissociable Intrinsic Connectivity Networks for Salience Processing and Executive Control , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[67]  V. Menon,et al.  A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[68]  G. Shulman,et al.  Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[69]  B. Harrison,et al.  Consistency and functional specialization in the default mode brain network , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[70]  E. Wagenmakers,et al.  A psychometric analysis of chess expertise. , 2005, The American journal of psychology.

[71]  P. Strick,et al.  Anatomical evidence for cerebellar and basal ganglia involvement in higher cognitive function. , 1994, Science.

[72]  Bharat B. Biswal,et al.  Competition between functional brain networks mediates behavioral variability , 2008, NeuroImage.

[73]  Thomas Elbert,et al.  Pattern of focal γ-bursts in chess players , 2001, Nature.

[74]  M. Raichle,et al.  Disease and the brain's dark energy , 2010, Nature Reviews Neurology.

[75]  Sheng He,et al.  A functional MRI study of high-level cognition. I. The game of chess. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[76]  Chaogan Yan,et al.  DPARSF: A MATLAB Toolbox for “Pipeline” Data Analysis of Resting-State fMRI , 2010, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[77]  K. Christoff,et al.  Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind wandering , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[78]  K. Davis,et al.  Two systems of resting state connectivity between the insula and cingulate cortex , 2009, Human brain mapping.