Pressure to communicate across knowledge asymmetries leads to pedagogically supportive language input

Children do not learn language from passive observation of the world, but from interaction with caregivers who want to communicate with them. These communicative exchanges are structured at multiple levels in ways that support support language learning. We argue this pedagogically supportive structure can result from pressure to communicate successfully with a linguistically immature partner. We first characterize one kind of pedagogically supportive structure in a corpus analysis: caregivers provide more information-rich referential communication, using both gesture and speech to refer to a single object, when that object is rare and when their child is young. Then, in an iterated reference game experiment on Mechanical Turk (n = 480), we show how this behavior can arise from pressure to communicate successfully with a less knowledgeable partner. Lastly, we show that speaker behavior in our experiment can be explained by a rational planning model, without any explicit teaching goal. We suggest that caregivers’ desire to communicate successfully may play a powerful role in structuring children’s input in order to support language learning.

[1]  D. Yurovsky A communicative approach to early word learning , 2017, New Ideas in Psychology.

[2]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Infants rapidly learn word-referent mappings via cross-situational statistics , 2008, Cognition.

[3]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Interpersonal Understanding Fuels Knowledge Acquisition , 2000 .

[4]  Elissa L. Newport,et al.  Maturational Constraints on Language Learning , 1990, Cogn. Sci..

[5]  Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Children gesture when speech is slow to come , 2018, CogSci.

[6]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  Linguistic input is tuned to children's developmental level , 2016, CogSci.

[7]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Memory constraints on infants’ cross-situational statistical learning , 2013, Cognition.

[8]  L. Gogate,et al.  A study of multimodal motherese: the role of temporal synchrony between verbal labels and gestures. , 2000, Child development.

[9]  H. Gleitman,et al.  Mother, Id rather do it myself: Some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style , 1977 .

[10]  A. Meltzoff 1 Imitation and Other Minds: The "Like Me" Hypothesis , 2005 .

[11]  Erik D. Thiessen,et al.  Infant-Directed Speech Facilitates Word Segmentation. , 2005, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[12]  E. Markman,et al.  Children's use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words , 1988, Cognitive Psychology.

[13]  F. Inglis How To Do Things With Words. , 1971 .

[14]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  Predicting Pragmatic Reasoning in Language Games , 2012, Science.

[15]  Paul Vogt Exploring the Robustness of Cross-Situational Learning Under Zipfian Distributions , 2012, Cogn. Sci..

[16]  J. Saffran Statistical Language Learning , 2003 .

[17]  M. Minami How Children Learn the Meanings of Words , 2001 .

[18]  Leslie Pack Kaelbling,et al.  Planning and Acting in Partially Observable Stochastic Domains , 1998, Artif. Intell..

[19]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Visual Attention Is Not Enough: Individual Differences in Statistical Word-Referent Learning in Infants , 2013, Language learning and development : the official journal of the Society for Language Development.

[20]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Joint attention and early language. , 1986, Child development.

[21]  Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al.  New Evidence about Language and Cognitive Development Based on a Longitudinal Study Hypotheses for Intervention Istrative and Technical Support. We Also Thank Varying the Language Learning Environment Susan Goldin- Meadow Varying the Language Learner Gesture: Another Perspective on Communicative Abi , 2022 .