Effects of Pulvinar Inactivation on Spatial Decision-making between Equal and Asymmetric Reward Options

The ability to selectively process visual inputs and to decide between multiple movement options in an adaptive manner is critical for survival. Such decisions are known to be influenced by factors such as reward expectation and visual saliency. The dorsal pulvinar connects to a multitude of cortical areas that are involved in visuospatial memory and integrate information about upcoming eye movements with expected reward values. However, it is unclear whether the dorsal pulvinar is critically involved in spatial memory and reward-based oculomotor decision behavior. To examine this, we reversibly inactivated the dorsal portion of the pulvinar while monkeys performed a delayed memory saccade task that included choices between equally or unequally rewarded options. Pulvinar inactivation resulted in a delay of saccade initiation toward memorized contralesional targets but did not affect spatial memory. Furthermore, pulvinar inactivation caused a pronounced choice bias toward the ipsilesional hemifield when the reward value in the two hemifields was equal. However, this choice bias could be alleviated by placing a high reward target into the contralesional hemifield. The bias was less affected by the manipulation of relative visual saliency between the two competing targets. These results suggest that the dorsal pulvinar is involved in determining the behavioral desirability of movement goals while being less critical for spatial memory and reward processing.

[1]  L. Pessoa,et al.  Interactions between cognition and motivation during response inhibition , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  H. Karnath,et al.  The subcortical anatomy of human spatial neglect: putamen, caudate nucleus and pulvinar. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[3]  Y. Saalmann,et al.  Cognitive and Perceptual Functions of the Visual Thalamus , 2011, Neuron.

[4]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Comparison of Reward Modulation in the Frontal Eye Field and Caudate of the Macaque , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  M. Posner,et al.  Deficits in human visual spatial attention following thalamic lesions. , 1987, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[6]  N. Logothetis,et al.  A combined MRI and histology atlas of the rhesus monkey brain in stereotaxic coordinates , 2007 .

[7]  Okihide Hikosaka,et al.  Role of Primate Substantia Nigra Pars Reticulata in Reward-Oriented Saccadic Eye Movement , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  M. Segraves,et al.  Muscimol-induced inactivation of monkey frontal eye field: effects on visually and memory-guided saccades. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Effect of Expected Reward Magnitude on the Response of Neurons in the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex of the Macaque , 1999, Neuron.

[10]  S. Sherman,et al.  Thalamic relays and cortical functioning. , 2005, Progress in brain research.

[11]  K. Grieve,et al.  The primate pulvinar nuclei: vision and action , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.

[12]  D. Pandya,et al.  Thalamic connections of the cortex of the superior temporal sulcus in the rhesus monkey , 1989, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[13]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Feature-Based Anticipation of Cues that Predict Reward in Monkey Caudate Nucleus , 2002, Neuron.

[14]  Patrik Vuilleumier,et al.  Emotional processing and its impact on unilateral neglect and extinction , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  Hans-Jochen Heinze,et al.  High-Field fMRI Reveals Brain Activation Patterns Underlying Saccade Execution in the Human Superior Colliculus , 2010, PloS one.

[16]  R. Dolan,et al.  Reward Facilitates Tactile Judgments and Modulates Hemodynamic Responses in Human Primary Somatosensory Cortex , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  D. B. Bender,et al.  Effect of attentive fixation in macaque thalamus and cortex. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  Distribution of activity across the monkey cerebral cortical surface, thalamus and midbrain during rapid, visually guided saccades. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[19]  Iwona Stepniewska,et al.  The Pulvinar Complex , 2003 .

[20]  Jon H. Kaas,et al.  Pulvinar contributions to the dorsal and ventral streams of visual processing in primates , 2007, Brain Research Reviews.

[21]  R. Andersen,et al.  Visual receptive field organization and cortico‐cortical connections of the lateral intraparietal area (area LIP) in the macaque , 1990, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[22]  Sabine Kastner,et al.  Functional imaging of the human lateral geniculate nucleus and pulvinar. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  R. Rafal,et al.  Oculomotor integration in patients with a pulvinar lesion , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  Robert H. Wurtz,et al.  Signals Conveyed in the Pulvinar Pathway from Superior Colliculus to Cortical Area MT , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Neural mechanisms for interacting with a world full of action choices. , 2010, Annual review of neuroscience.

[26]  C. A. Marzi,et al.  Input and Response Determinants of Visual Extinction: A Case Study , 1996, Cortex.

[27]  A. Grabowska,et al.  Emotionally negative stimuli can overcome attentional deficits in patients with visuo-spatial hemineglect , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  Robert Ward,et al.  The Effects of Unilateral Pulvinar Damage in Humans on Reflexive Orienting and Filtering of Irrelevant Information , 2002, Behavioural neurology.

[29]  L. Chelazzi,et al.  Behavioral/systems/cognitive Reward Changes Salience in Human Vision via the Anterior Cingulate , 2022 .

[30]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Common cortical and subcortical targets of the dorsolateral prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices in the rhesus monkey: evidence for a distributed neural network subserving spatially guided behavior , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[31]  C. Kennard,et al.  The anatomy of visual neglect. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[32]  J. C. Crowley,et al.  Saccade Reward Signals in Posterior Cingulate Cortex , 2003, Neuron.

[33]  P. H. Schiller,et al.  The effects of frontal eye field and dorsomedial frontal cortex lesions on visually guided eye movements , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[34]  J. Duhamel,et al.  Saccadic Target Selection Deficits after Lateral Intraparietal Area Inactivation in Monkeys , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  Pia Rotshtein,et al.  Pleasant music overcomes the loss of awareness in patients with visual neglect , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[36]  Shintaro Funahashi,et al.  Prefrontal task-related activity representing visual cue location or saccade direction in spatial working memory tasks. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  C. Bruce,et al.  Neural circuitry of judgment and decision mechanisms , 2005, Brain Research Reviews.

[38]  R. Dolan,et al.  Dopamine and Effort-Based Decision Making , 2011, Front. Neurosci..

[39]  L. Pessoa,et al.  Pulvinar and Affective Significance: Responses Track Moment-to-Moment Stimulus Visibility , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[40]  A. Song,et al.  The involvement of the dopaminergic midbrain and cortico-striatal-thalamic circuits in the integration of reward prospect and attentional task demands. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[41]  Yq Liu,et al.  Intention and Attention: Different functional roles for LIPd and LIPv , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[42]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Matching patterns of activity in primate prefrontal area 8a and parietal area 7ip neurons during a spatial working memory task. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  S. Petersen,et al.  The pulvinar and visual salience , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[44]  Tony Ro,et al.  The role of the human pulvinar in visual attention and action: evidence from temporal-order judgment, saccade decision, and antisaccade tasks. , 2008, Progress in brain research.

[45]  S E Petersen,et al.  Visual responses of pulvinar and collicular neurons during eye movements of awake, trained macaques. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  R. Andersen,et al.  Functional imaging reveals rapid reorganization of cortical activity after parietal inactivation in monkeys , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[47]  Louise Whiteley,et al.  Spatially Distributed Encoding of Covert Attentional Shifts in Human Thalamus , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  Robert Ward,et al.  Spatial and temporal deficits are regionally dissociable in patients with pulvinar lesions. , 2008, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[49]  P Vuilleumier,et al.  Neural response to emotional faces with and without awareness: event-related fMRI in a parietal patient with visual extinction and spatial neglect , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[50]  R. Andersen,et al.  Effect of reversible inactivation of macaque lateral intraparietal area on visual and memory saccades. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[51]  E. De Renzi,et al.  An experimental investigation on the nature of extinction. , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[52]  Michael L. Platt,et al.  Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex , 1999, Nature.

[53]  Janita Turchi,et al.  Pulvinar Inactivation Disrupts Selection of Movement Plans , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  R. Andersen,et al.  The thalamic relations of the caudal inferior parietal lobule and the lateral prefrontal cortex in monkeys: Divergent cortical projections from cell clusters in the medial pulvinar nucleus , 1985, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[55]  R. Andersen,et al.  Memory related motor planning activity in posterior parietal cortex of macaque , 1988, Experimental Brain Research.

[56]  S. Petersen,et al.  Pulvinar nuclei of the behaving rhesus monkey: visual responses and their modulation. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[57]  E. Fox Processing emotional facial expressions: The role of anxiety and awareness , 2002, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[58]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Modulation of saccadic eye movements by predicted reward outcome , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[59]  L. Pessoa,et al.  Motivation sharpens exogenous spatial attention. , 2007, Emotion.

[60]  L. Pessoa,et al.  Embedding Reward Signals into Perception and Cognition , 2010, Front. Neurosci..

[61]  David M Milstein,et al.  The Influence of Expected Value on Saccadic Preparation , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[62]  Jon Driver,et al.  Integration of Goal- and Stimulus-Related Visual Signals Revealed by Damage to Human Parietal Cortex , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[63]  B. Seltzer,et al.  Neurochemical and connectional organization of the dorsal pulvinar complex in monkeys , 2000, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[64]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Dorsolateral prefrontal lesions and oculomotor delayed-response performance: evidence for mnemonic "scotomas" , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[65]  T D Albright,et al.  Effects of superior temporal polysensory area lesions on eye movements in the macaque monkey. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[66]  J Zihl,et al.  The contribution of the 'second' visual system to directed visual attention in man. , 1979, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[67]  D. Leopold,et al.  Neural activity in the visual thalamus reflects perceptual suppression , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[68]  F Mauguiere,et al.  Anatomical evidence for medial pulvinar connections with the posterior cingulate cortex, the retrosplenial area, and the posterior parahippocampal gyrus in monkeys , 1985, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[69]  D. Robinson,et al.  Orbital position and eye movement influences on visual responses in the pulvinar nuclei of the behaving macaque , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[70]  M. Platt,et al.  Expectations and outcomes: decision-making in the primate brain , 2005, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[71]  M. Kinsbourne Hemi-neglect and hemisphere rivalry. , 1977, Advances in neurology.

[72]  Patrik Vuilleumier,et al.  Emotional facial expressions capture attention , 2001, Neurology.

[73]  L. Benevento,et al.  Single neurons with both form/color differential responses and saccade-related responses in the nonretinotopic pulvinar of the behaving macaque monkey , 1995, Visual Neuroscience.

[74]  S. Petersen,et al.  Contributions of the pulvinar to visual spatial attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[75]  M. Mesulam Spatial attention and neglect: parietal, frontal and cingulate contributions to the mental representation and attentional targeting of salient extrapersonal events. , 1999, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[76]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  A neural correlate of response bias in monkey caudate nucleus , 2002, Nature.

[77]  E. M. Rouiller,et al.  Thalamocortical and the dual pattern of corticothalamic projections of the posterior parietal cortex in macaque monkeys , 2007, Neuroscience.

[78]  R. Andersen,et al.  Space representation for eye movements is more contralateral in monkeys than in humans , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.