Representations of motion and direction.

In 6 experiments, incidental memory was tested for direction of motion in an old-new recognition paradigm. Ability to recognize previously shown directions depended greatly on motion type. Memory for translation and expansion-contraction direction was highly veridical, whereas memory for rotation direction was conspicuously absent. Similar results were obtained in conditions in which motions were illustrated with pictures. Results suggest that explicit representations of direction in long-term memory are not so much related to motion per se as to the consequences of motion, the displacements of objects. Memory for all motions following circular pathways was found to be corrupted by a generic bias to regard the clockwise direction as familiar. Assessment of memory in these cases required disentangling familiarity bias for the clockwise direction from explicit recognition of direction.

[1]  R. Shepard,et al.  Comparison of cube rotations around axes inclined relative to the environment or to the cube. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  E. Reed The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1989 .

[3]  M. Graziano,et al.  Tuning of MST neurons to spiral motions , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[4]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical pathways , 1983, Trends in Neurosciences.

[5]  BELA JULESZ,et al.  Inability to Perceive the Direction of Rotation Movement of Line Segments , 1970, Nature.

[6]  Charles G. Gross,et al.  Pattern recognition mechanisms , 1985 .

[7]  K. Tanaka,et al.  Analysis of object motion in the ventral part of the medial superior temporal area of the macaque visual cortex. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  T. Hubbard,et al.  Environmental invariants in the representation of motion: Implied dynamics and representational momentum, gravity, friction, and centripetal force , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[9]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Integration of direction signals of image motion in the superior temporal sulcus of the macaque monkey , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[10]  E. Adelson,et al.  Phenomenal coherence of moving visual patterns , 1982, Nature.

[11]  K. Tanaka,et al.  Analysis of motion of the visual field by direction, expansion/contraction, and rotation cells clustered in the dorsal part of the medial superior temporal area of the macaque monkey. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  J. Cutting,et al.  Perception of wheel-generated motions. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  H. Hecht,et al.  Influence of animation on dynamical judgments. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Psychophysical isolation of movement sensitivity by removal of familiar position cues , 1981, Vision Research.

[15]  A J Ahumada,et al.  Model of human visual-motion sensing. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[16]  Christopher Habel,et al.  Spatial Reference Systems , 1999, Spatial Cogn. Comput..

[17]  J. Bharucha,et al.  Judged displacement in apparent vertical and horizontal motion , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[18]  J. Pani Limits on the Comprehension of Rotational Motion: Mental Imagery of Rotations with Oblique Components , 1993, Perception.

[19]  Gordon T. Shippey,et al.  Determinants of the Perception of Rotational Motion : Orientation of the Motion to the Object and to the Environment , 1995 .

[20]  J. Freyd,et al.  A velocity effect for representational momentum , 1985 .

[21]  M. Tarr Rotating objects to recognize them: A case study on the role of viewpoint dependency in the recognition of three-dimensional objects , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[22]  J R Pani,et al.  Spatial Reference Systems in the Comprehension of Rotational Motion , 1994, Perception.

[23]  G. Schneider Two visual systems. , 1969, Science.

[24]  R. Blake,et al.  Memory for visual motion. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  H. Hecht Judging Rolling Wheels: Dynamic and Kinematic Aspects of Rotation – Translation Coupling , 1993, Perception.

[26]  A. Glenberg,et al.  What memory is for: Creating meaning in the service of action , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[27]  T. Takeuchi,et al.  Visual search of expansion and contraction , 1997, Vision Research.

[28]  E H Adelson,et al.  Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[29]  Ken Nakayama,et al.  Serial and parallel processing of visual feature conjunctions , 1986, Nature.

[30]  M. H. Kelly,et al.  Memory biases in left versus right implied motion. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[31]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[32]  O. Braddick,et al.  Serial Search for Targets Defined by Divergence or Deformation of Optic Flow , 1991, Perception.

[33]  T. Hubbard Cognitive representation of linear motion: Possible direction and gravity effects in judged displacement , 1990, Memory & cognition.

[34]  E. Adelson,et al.  The analysis of moving visual patterns , 1985 .