Motor abstraction: a neuroscientific account of how action goals and intentions are mapped and understood
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] L. Fogassi,et al. Audiovisual mirror neurons and action recognition , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.
[2] Alvin I. Goldman,et al. Philosophical Applications Of Cognitive Science , 1983 .
[3] D. Premack. The infant's theory of self-propelled objects , 1990, Cognition.
[4] V. Gallese,et al. Cognitive Continuity in Primate Social Cognition , 2006 .
[5] R. Passingham,et al. Action observation and acquired motor skills: an FMRI study with expert dancers. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.
[6] G. Csibra,et al. Goal attribution without agency cues: the perception of ‘pure reason’ in infancy , 1999, Cognition.
[7] A. Meltzoff. Understanding the Intentions of Others: Re-Enactment of Intended Acts by 18-Month-Old Children. , 1995, Developmental psychology.
[8] G. Rizzolatti,et al. The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.
[9] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[10] R. Johansson,et al. Action plans used in action observation , 2003, Nature.
[11] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Afferent properties of periarcuate neurons in macaque monkeys. II. Visual responses , 1981, Behavioural Brain Research.
[12] G Rizzolatti,et al. When pliers become fingers in the monkey motor system , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[13] Riitta Hari,et al. Activation of the human primary motor cortex during observation of tool use , 2004, NeuroImage.
[14] Dare A. Baldwin,et al. Intentions and Intentionality: Foundations of Social Cognition , 2001 .
[15] M. Gazzaniga. The new cognitive neurosciences, 2nd ed. , 2000 .
[16] V. Gallese. The 'shared manifold' hypothesis: From mirror neurons to empathy. , 2001 .
[17] L. Buxbaum,et al. On beyond mirror neurons: internal representations subserving imitation and recognition of skilled object-related actions in humans. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[18] L. Fogassi,et al. Functional properties of grasping-related neurons in the ventral premotor area F5 of the macaque monkey. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[19] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Object representation in the ventral premotor cortex (area F5) of the monkey. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.
[20] T. Metzinger,et al. The emergence of a shared action ontology: Building blocks for a theory , 2003, Consciousness and Cognition.
[21] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Speech listening specifically modulates the excitability of tongue muscles: a TMS study , 2002, The European journal of neuroscience.
[22] V. Gallese. Intentional attunement: A neurophysiological perspective on social cognition and its disruption in autism , 2006, Brain Research.
[23] V. Gallese,et al. Embodied simulation: From neurons to phenomenal experience , 2005 .
[24] Vittorio Gallese,et al. The eyes, the hand, and the mind: behavioral and neurophysiological aspects of social cognition , 2001 .
[25] Jodie A. Baird,et al. Infants parse dynamic action. , 2001, Child development.
[26] S. Hurley. Consciousness in Action , 2000 .
[27] Thomas Metzinger,et al. The Subjectivity of Subjective Experience: A Representationalist Analysis of the First-Person Perspective. , 2000 .
[28] M. Hepp-Reymond,et al. Force-related neuronal activity in two regions of the primate ventral premotor cortex. , 1994, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.
[29] H. Bekkering,et al. Developmental psychology: Rational imitation in preverbal infants , 2002, Nature.
[30] G. Rizzolatti,et al. I Know What You Are Doing A Neurophysiological Study , 2001, Neuron.
[31] Nadim Joni Shah,et al. Prefrontal involvement in imitation learning of hand actions: Effects of practice and expertise , 2007, NeuroImage.
[32] J. Tanji,et al. Premotor cortex neurons in macaques: activity before distal and proximal forelimb movements , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[33] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Parietal Lobe: From Action Organization to Intention Understanding , 2005, Science.
[34] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[35] A. Leslie. Mapping the mind: ToMM, ToBY, and Agency: Core architecture and domain specificity , 1994 .
[36] S. Baron-Cohen. How to build a baby that can read minds: Cognitive mechanisms in mindreading. , 1994 .
[37] Vittorio Gallese,et al. Reply to Schulkin , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[38] J. Mazziotta,et al. Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. , 1999, Science.
[39] Maurice Merleau-Ponty. Phenomenology of Perception , 1964 .
[40] J. J. Guajardo,et al. How infants make sense of intentional action , 2001 .
[41] Alvin I. Goldman,et al. The Mentalizing Folk , 2002 .
[42] Thomas Metzinger,et al. Motor ontology: the representational reality of goals, actions and selves , 2003 .
[43] S. Gelman,et al. Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity In Cognition And Culture , 1994 .
[44] A. Goldman. In Defense of the Simulation Theory , 1992 .
[45] G. Rizzolatti,et al. A unifying view of the basis of social cognition , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[46] V. Gallese. The Inner Sense of Action , 2000 .
[47] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Action recognition in the premotor cortex. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[48] H. Bekkering,et al. Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[49] Thomas Metzinger,et al. Conference on Neural Correlates of Consciousness: Empirical and Conceptual Questions , 1998, Consciousness and Cognition.
[50] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Neural Circuits Underlying Imitation Learning of Hand Actions An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2004, Neuron.
[51] Vittorio Gallese,et al. 31 Cortical Mechanisms Subserving Object Grasping, Action Understanding, and Imitation , 2004 .
[52] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.
[53] R. Gordon. Sellars’s Ryleans Revisited , 2000 .
[54] G. Lakoff,et al. The Brain's concepts: the role of the Sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[55] Empathy, Mind, and Morals , 1992 .
[56] C. Keysers,et al. Integrating simulation and theory of mind: from self to social cognition , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[57] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Functional organization of inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[58] J. Mazziotta,et al. Grasping the Intentions of Others with One's Own Mirror Neuron System , 2005, PLoS biology.
[59] Peter K. Smith,et al. Theories of Theories of Mind , 1996 .
[60] C. Hofsten,et al. Infants predict other people's action goals , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.
[61] V. Gallese. Empathy, Embodied Simulation, and the Brain: Commentary on Aragno and Zepf/Hartmann , 2008, Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association.
[62] Vittorio Gallese,et al. A neuroscientific grasp of concepts: from control to representation. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[63] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Afferent properties of periarcuate neurons in macaque monkeys. I. Somatosensory responses , 1981, Behavioural Brain Research.
[64] J. Danckert. Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action: Attention and Performance XIX Wolfgang Prinz, Bernhard Hommel (Eds.), Oxford University Press, 2002, Price: £ 65.00, ISBN: 0-19-851069 , 2003, Neuropsychologia.
[65] V. Gallese. Before and below ‘theory of mind’: embodied simulation and the neural correlates of social cognition , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[66] R. Gordon,et al. Theories of theories of mind: ‘Radical’ simulationism , 1996 .
[67] V. Gallese,et al. Mirror and canonical neurons are crucial elements in esthetic response , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[68] A. Goldman,et al. Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[69] Jessica A. Sommerville,et al. Pulling out the intentional structure of action: the relation between action processing and action production in infancy , 2005, Cognition.
[70] V. Gallese. The Roots of Empathy: The Shared Manifold Hypothesis and the Neural Basis of Intersubjectivity , 2003, Psychopathology.
[71] B. Landau,et al. Starting at the end: the importance of goals in spatial language , 2005, Cognition.
[72] L. Fadiga,et al. The Evolution of Social Cognition: Goal Familiarity Shapes Monkeys' Action Understanding , 2008, Current Biology.
[73] M. Iacoboni,et al. The self and social cognition: the role of cortical midline structures and mirror neurons , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[74] R. Gordon. Folk Psychology as Simulation , 1986 .
[75] V. Gallese. Action representaion and the inferior parietal lobule , 2000 .
[76] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Patterns of cytochrome oxidase activity in the frontal agranular cortex of the macaque monkey , 1985, Behavioural Brain Research.
[77] Thomas Metzinger. Subjekt und Selbstmodell , 1993 .
[78] J. Sommerville,et al. Action experience alters 3-month-old infants' perception of others' actions , 2005, Cognition.
[79] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Grasping objects and grasping action meanings: the dual role of monkey rostroventral premotor cortex (area F5). , 1998, Novartis Foundation symposium.
[80] L. Fogassi,et al. A Touching Sight SII/PV Activation during the Observation and Experience of Touch , 2004, Neuron.
[81] M. Arbib,et al. Grasping objects: the cortical mechanisms of visuomotor transformation , 1995, Trends in Neurosciences.
[82] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Neural Circuits Involved in the Recognition of Actions Performed by Nonconspecifics: An fMRI Study , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[83] D. Stainier,et al. A Cellular Framework for Gut-Looping Morphogenesis in Zebrafish , 2003, Science.
[84] V. Gallese. The manifold nature of interpersonal relations: the quest for a common mechanism. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[85] Z. Nadasdy,et al. Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age , 1995, Cognition.
[86] S-J Blakemore,et al. Somatosensory activations during the observation of touch and a case of vision-touch synaesthesia. , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[87] V. Gallese,et al. Intentional Attunement: Mirror Neurons and the Neural Underpinnings of Interpersonal Relations , 2007, Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association.
[88] R. Passingham,et al. Seeing or Doing? Influence of Visual and Motor Familiarity in Action Observation , 2006, Current Biology.
[89] Jonas Langer,et al. Biology and knowledge revisited : from neurogenesis to psychogenesis , 2005 .
[90] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Hearing Sounds, Understanding Actions: Action Representation in Mirror Neurons , 2002, Science.
[91] V. Gallese,et al. Motor cognition and its role in the phylogeny and ontogeny of action understanding. , 2009, Developmental psychology.
[92] György Gergely,et al. One-year-old infants use teleological representations of actions productively , 2003, Cogn. Sci..
[93] V. Gallese,et al. Motion, emotion and empathy in esthetic experience , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[94] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Congruent Embodied Representations for Visually Presented Actions and Linguistic Phrases Describing Actions , 2006, Current Biology.
[95] Vittorio Gallese,et al. The Sense of Touch: Embodied Simulation in a Visuotactile Mirroring Mechanism for Observed Animate or Inanimate Touch , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[96] V. Gallese. The ‘‘shared manifold’’ hypothesis: from mirror neurons to empathy , 2001 .
[97] G. Csibra,et al. Teleological reasoning in infancy: the naı̈ve theory of rational action , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[98] S. Tipper,et al. Vision-for-action: The effects of object property discrimination and action state on affordance compatibility effects , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[99] H. Preester. From ego to alter ego: Husserl, Merleau-Ponty and a layered approach to intersubjectivity , 2008 .
[100] M. Tomasello,et al. Twelve- and 18-month-olds copy actions in terms of goals. , 2005, Developmental science.
[101] George S. Cree,et al. Evocation of functional and volumetric gestural knowledge by objects and words , 2008, Cognition.
[102] A. Goldman. The psychology of folk psychology , 1993, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[103] T Brochier,et al. Simultaneous recording of macaque premotor and primary motor cortex neuronal populations reveals different functional contributions to visuomotor grasp. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.
[104] Wolfgang Prinz,et al. Off-line authorship effects in action perception , 2003, Brain and Cognition.
[105] R. Millikan. Varieties of Meaning: The 2002 Jean Nicod Lectures , 2004 .
[106] Jodie A. Baird,et al. Making sense of human behavior: Action parsing and intentional inference , 2001 .
[107] Kenneth Williford,et al. Neural Correlates of Consciousness: Empirical and Conceptual Questions , 2005, Minds and Machines.