Motor abstraction: a neuroscientific account of how action goals and intentions are mapped and understood

Recent findings in cognitive neuroscience shed light on the existence of a common neural mechanism that could account for action and intention to understand abilities in humans and non-human primates. Empirical evidence on the neural underpinnings of action goals and on their ontogeny and phylogeny is introduced and discussed. It is proposed that the properties of the mirror neuron system and the functional mechanism describing them, embodied simulation, enabled pre-linguistic forms of action and intention understanding. Basic aspects of social cognition appear to be primarily based on the motor cognition that underpins one’s own capacity to act, here defined as motor abstraction. On the basis of this new account of the motor system, it is proposed that intersubjectivity is the best conceived of as intercorporeity.

[1]  L. Fogassi,et al.  Audiovisual mirror neurons and action recognition , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  Alvin I. Goldman,et al.  Philosophical Applications Of Cognitive Science , 1983 .

[3]  D. Premack The infant's theory of self-propelled objects , 1990, Cognition.

[4]  V. Gallese,et al.  Cognitive Continuity in Primate Social Cognition , 2006 .

[5]  R. Passingham,et al.  Action observation and acquired motor skills: an FMRI study with expert dancers. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[6]  G. Csibra,et al.  Goal attribution without agency cues: the perception of ‘pure reason’ in infancy , 1999, Cognition.

[7]  A. Meltzoff Understanding the Intentions of Others: Re-Enactment of Intended Acts by 18-Month-Old Children. , 1995, Developmental psychology.

[8]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[9]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[10]  R. Johansson,et al.  Action plans used in action observation , 2003, Nature.

[11]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Afferent properties of periarcuate neurons in macaque monkeys. II. Visual responses , 1981, Behavioural Brain Research.

[12]  G Rizzolatti,et al.  When pliers become fingers in the monkey motor system , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[13]  Riitta Hari,et al.  Activation of the human primary motor cortex during observation of tool use , 2004, NeuroImage.

[14]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Intentions and Intentionality: Foundations of Social Cognition , 2001 .

[15]  M. Gazzaniga The new cognitive neurosciences, 2nd ed. , 2000 .

[16]  V. Gallese The 'shared manifold' hypothesis: From mirror neurons to empathy. , 2001 .

[17]  L. Buxbaum,et al.  On beyond mirror neurons: internal representations subserving imitation and recognition of skilled object-related actions in humans. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[18]  L. Fogassi,et al.  Functional properties of grasping-related neurons in the ventral premotor area F5 of the macaque monkey. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Object representation in the ventral premotor cortex (area F5) of the monkey. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  T. Metzinger,et al.  The emergence of a shared action ontology: Building blocks for a theory , 2003, Consciousness and Cognition.

[21]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Speech listening specifically modulates the excitability of tongue muscles: a TMS study , 2002, The European journal of neuroscience.

[22]  V. Gallese Intentional attunement: A neurophysiological perspective on social cognition and its disruption in autism , 2006, Brain Research.

[23]  V. Gallese,et al.  Embodied simulation: From neurons to phenomenal experience , 2005 .

[24]  Vittorio Gallese,et al.  The eyes, the hand, and the mind: behavioral and neurophysiological aspects of social cognition , 2001 .

[25]  Jodie A. Baird,et al.  Infants parse dynamic action. , 2001, Child development.

[26]  S. Hurley Consciousness in Action , 2000 .

[27]  Thomas Metzinger,et al.  The Subjectivity of Subjective Experience: A Representationalist Analysis of the First-Person Perspective. , 2000 .

[28]  M. Hepp-Reymond,et al.  Force-related neuronal activity in two regions of the primate ventral premotor cortex. , 1994, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[29]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Developmental psychology: Rational imitation in preverbal infants , 2002, Nature.

[30]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  I Know What You Are Doing A Neurophysiological Study , 2001, Neuron.

[31]  Nadim Joni Shah,et al.  Prefrontal involvement in imitation learning of hand actions: Effects of practice and expertise , 2007, NeuroImage.

[32]  J. Tanji,et al.  Premotor cortex neurons in macaques: activity before distal and proximal forelimb movements , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[33]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Parietal Lobe: From Action Organization to Intention Understanding , 2005, Science.

[34]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[35]  A. Leslie Mapping the mind: ToMM, ToBY, and Agency: Core architecture and domain specificity , 1994 .

[36]  S. Baron-Cohen How to build a baby that can read minds: Cognitive mechanisms in mindreading. , 1994 .

[37]  Vittorio Gallese,et al.  Reply to Schulkin , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[38]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. , 1999, Science.

[39]  Maurice Merleau-Ponty Phenomenology of Perception , 1964 .

[40]  J. J. Guajardo,et al.  How infants make sense of intentional action , 2001 .

[41]  Alvin I. Goldman,et al.  The Mentalizing Folk , 2002 .

[42]  Thomas Metzinger,et al.  Motor ontology: the representational reality of goals, actions and selves , 2003 .

[43]  S. Gelman,et al.  Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity In Cognition And Culture , 1994 .

[44]  A. Goldman In Defense of the Simulation Theory , 1992 .

[45]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  A unifying view of the basis of social cognition , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[46]  V. Gallese The Inner Sense of Action , 2000 .

[47]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action recognition in the premotor cortex. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[48]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[49]  Thomas Metzinger,et al.  Conference on Neural Correlates of Consciousness: Empirical and Conceptual Questions , 1998, Consciousness and Cognition.

[50]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neural Circuits Underlying Imitation Learning of Hand Actions An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2004, Neuron.

[51]  Vittorio Gallese,et al.  31 Cortical Mechanisms Subserving Object Grasping, Action Understanding, and Imitation , 2004 .

[52]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[53]  R. Gordon Sellars’s Ryleans Revisited , 2000 .

[54]  G. Lakoff,et al.  The Brain's concepts: the role of the Sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[55]  Empathy, Mind, and Morals , 1992 .

[56]  C. Keysers,et al.  Integrating simulation and theory of mind: from self to social cognition , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[57]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Functional organization of inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[58]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Grasping the Intentions of Others with One's Own Mirror Neuron System , 2005, PLoS biology.

[59]  Peter K. Smith,et al.  Theories of Theories of Mind , 1996 .

[60]  C. Hofsten,et al.  Infants predict other people's action goals , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[61]  V. Gallese Empathy, Embodied Simulation, and the Brain: Commentary on Aragno and Zepf/Hartmann , 2008, Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association.

[62]  Vittorio Gallese,et al.  A neuroscientific grasp of concepts: from control to representation. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[63]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Afferent properties of periarcuate neurons in macaque monkeys. I. Somatosensory responses , 1981, Behavioural Brain Research.

[64]  J. Danckert Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action: Attention and Performance XIX Wolfgang Prinz, Bernhard Hommel (Eds.), Oxford University Press, 2002, Price: £ 65.00, ISBN: 0-19-851069 , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[65]  V. Gallese Before and below ‘theory of mind’: embodied simulation and the neural correlates of social cognition , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[66]  R. Gordon,et al.  Theories of theories of mind: ‘Radical’ simulationism , 1996 .

[67]  V. Gallese,et al.  Mirror and canonical neurons are crucial elements in esthetic response , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[68]  A. Goldman,et al.  Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[69]  Jessica A. Sommerville,et al.  Pulling out the intentional structure of action: the relation between action processing and action production in infancy , 2005, Cognition.

[70]  V. Gallese The Roots of Empathy: The Shared Manifold Hypothesis and the Neural Basis of Intersubjectivity , 2003, Psychopathology.

[71]  B. Landau,et al.  Starting at the end: the importance of goals in spatial language , 2005, Cognition.

[72]  L. Fadiga,et al.  The Evolution of Social Cognition: Goal Familiarity Shapes Monkeys' Action Understanding , 2008, Current Biology.

[73]  M. Iacoboni,et al.  The self and social cognition: the role of cortical midline structures and mirror neurons , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[74]  R. Gordon Folk Psychology as Simulation , 1986 .

[75]  V. Gallese Action representaion and the inferior parietal lobule , 2000 .

[76]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Patterns of cytochrome oxidase activity in the frontal agranular cortex of the macaque monkey , 1985, Behavioural Brain Research.

[77]  Thomas Metzinger Subjekt und Selbstmodell , 1993 .

[78]  J. Sommerville,et al.  Action experience alters 3-month-old infants' perception of others' actions , 2005, Cognition.

[79]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Grasping objects and grasping action meanings: the dual role of monkey rostroventral premotor cortex (area F5). , 1998, Novartis Foundation symposium.

[80]  L. Fogassi,et al.  A Touching Sight SII/PV Activation during the Observation and Experience of Touch , 2004, Neuron.

[81]  M. Arbib,et al.  Grasping objects: the cortical mechanisms of visuomotor transformation , 1995, Trends in Neurosciences.

[82]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neural Circuits Involved in the Recognition of Actions Performed by Nonconspecifics: An fMRI Study , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[83]  D. Stainier,et al.  A Cellular Framework for Gut-Looping Morphogenesis in Zebrafish , 2003, Science.

[84]  V. Gallese The manifold nature of interpersonal relations: the quest for a common mechanism. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[85]  Z. Nadasdy,et al.  Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age , 1995, Cognition.

[86]  S-J Blakemore,et al.  Somatosensory activations during the observation of touch and a case of vision-touch synaesthesia. , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[87]  V. Gallese,et al.  Intentional Attunement: Mirror Neurons and the Neural Underpinnings of Interpersonal Relations , 2007, Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association.

[88]  R. Passingham,et al.  Seeing or Doing? Influence of Visual and Motor Familiarity in Action Observation , 2006, Current Biology.

[89]  Jonas Langer,et al.  Biology and knowledge revisited : from neurogenesis to psychogenesis , 2005 .

[90]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Hearing Sounds, Understanding Actions: Action Representation in Mirror Neurons , 2002, Science.

[91]  V. Gallese,et al.  Motor cognition and its role in the phylogeny and ontogeny of action understanding. , 2009, Developmental psychology.

[92]  György Gergely,et al.  One-year-old infants use teleological representations of actions productively , 2003, Cogn. Sci..

[93]  V. Gallese,et al.  Motion, emotion and empathy in esthetic experience , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[94]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Congruent Embodied Representations for Visually Presented Actions and Linguistic Phrases Describing Actions , 2006, Current Biology.

[95]  Vittorio Gallese,et al.  The Sense of Touch: Embodied Simulation in a Visuotactile Mirroring Mechanism for Observed Animate or Inanimate Touch , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[96]  V. Gallese The ‘‘shared manifold’’ hypothesis: from mirror neurons to empathy , 2001 .

[97]  G. Csibra,et al.  Teleological reasoning in infancy: the naı̈ve theory of rational action , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[98]  S. Tipper,et al.  Vision-for-action: The effects of object property discrimination and action state on affordance compatibility effects , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[99]  H. Preester From ego to alter ego: Husserl, Merleau-Ponty and a layered approach to intersubjectivity , 2008 .

[100]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Twelve- and 18-month-olds copy actions in terms of goals. , 2005, Developmental science.

[101]  George S. Cree,et al.  Evocation of functional and volumetric gestural knowledge by objects and words , 2008, Cognition.

[102]  A. Goldman The psychology of folk psychology , 1993, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[103]  T Brochier,et al.  Simultaneous recording of macaque premotor and primary motor cortex neuronal populations reveals different functional contributions to visuomotor grasp. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[104]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  Off-line authorship effects in action perception , 2003, Brain and Cognition.

[105]  R. Millikan Varieties of Meaning: The 2002 Jean Nicod Lectures , 2004 .

[106]  Jodie A. Baird,et al.  Making sense of human behavior: Action parsing and intentional inference , 2001 .

[107]  Kenneth Williford,et al.  Neural Correlates of Consciousness: Empirical and Conceptual Questions , 2005, Minds and Machines.