Sociality influences cultural complexity

Archaeological and ethnohistorical evidence suggests a link between a population's size and structure, and the diversity or sophistication of its toolkits or technologies. Addressing these patterns, several evolutionary models predict that both the size and social interconnectedness of populations can contribute to the complexity of its cultural repertoire. Some models also predict that a sudden loss of sociality or of population will result in subsequent losses of useful skills/technologies. Here, we test these predictions with two experiments that permit learners to access either one or five models (teachers). Experiment 1 demonstrates that naive participants who could observe five models, integrate this information and generate increasingly effective skills (using an image editing tool) over 10 laboratory generations, whereas those with access to only one model show no improvement. Experiment 2, which began with a generation of trained experts, shows how learners with access to only one model lose skills (in knot-tying) more rapidly than those with access to five models. In the final generation of both experiments, all participants with access to five models demonstrate superior skills to those with access to only one model. These results support theoretical predictions linking sociality to cumulative cultural evolution.

[1]  Mark Collard,et al.  Causes of toolkit variation among hunter-gatherers: a test of four competing hypotheses , 2005 .

[2]  P. Marquet,et al.  Emergence of social complexity among coastal hunter-gatherers in the Atacama Desert of northern Chile , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[3]  M. Feldman,et al.  Gene-culture coevolutionary theory. , 1996, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[4]  Mark Nielsen,et al.  Imitation, pretend play, and childhood: essential elements in the evolution of human culture? , 2012, Journal of comparative psychology.

[5]  Herbert Gintis,et al.  Gene–culture coevolution and the nature of human sociality , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[6]  P. Harris,et al.  Stick to the script: The effect of witnessing multiple actors on children’s imitation , 2013, Cognition.

[7]  Lewis R. Binford,et al.  Constructing Frames of Reference: An Analytical Method for Archaeological Theory Building Using Ethnographic and Environmental Data Sets , 2001 .

[8]  P. Richerson,et al.  Culture and the Evolutionary Process , 1988 .

[9]  Joseph Henrich,et al.  The evolution of costly displays, cooperation and religion: credibility enhancing displays and their implications for cultural evolution , 2009 .

[10]  J. Henrich Demography and Cultural Evolution: How Adaptive Cultural Processes Can Produce Maladaptive Losses—The Tasmanian Case , 2004, American Antiquity.

[11]  D. Wilkins,et al.  The assessment of surgical skills and a simple knot-tying exercise. , 2001, Annals of the Royal College of Surgeons of England.

[12]  Michelle Y. Merrill,et al.  Orangutan Cultures and the Evolution of Material Culture , 2003, Science.

[13]  Carel P van Schaik,et al.  Social organization and the evolution of cumulative technology in apes and hominins. , 2012, Journal of human evolution.

[14]  C. V. van Schaik,et al.  A model for tool-use traditions in primates: implications for the coevolution of culture and cognition. , 2003, Journal of human evolution.

[15]  J. Henrich,et al.  Culture–gene coevolution, norm-psychology and the emergence of human prosociality , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  Robin I. M. Dunbar Neocortex size as a constraint on group size in primates , 1992 .

[17]  R. Boyd,et al.  Population size predicts technological complexity in Oceania , 2010, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[18]  B. Chapais Primeval Kinship: How Pair-Bonding Gave Birth to Human Society , 2008 .

[19]  T. Camplin Wired for Culture: Origins of the Human Social Mind , 2015 .

[20]  N. Chagnon,et al.  Life Histories, Blood Revenge, and Warfare in a Tribal Population , 1988, Science.

[21]  K. Laland,et al.  How culture shaped the human genome: bringing genetics and the human sciences together , 2010, Nature Reviews Genetics.

[22]  B. Hewlett,et al.  Co-Residence Patterns in Hunter-Gatherer Societies Show Unique Human Social Structure , 2011, Science.

[23]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Humans Have Evolved Specialized Skills of Social Cognition: The Cultural Intelligence Hypothesis , 2007, Science.

[24]  Judith M Burkart,et al.  Social learning and evolution: the cultural intelligence hypothesis , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[25]  J. Henrich The evolution of innovation-enhancing institutions , 2007 .

[26]  P. Goldberg,et al.  Middle Stone Age Bedding Construction and Settlement Patterns at Sibudu, South Africa , 2011, Science.

[27]  N. Christakis,et al.  Social Networks and Cooperation in Hunter-Gatherers , 2011, Nature.

[28]  Magnus Enquist,et al.  One cultural parent makes no culture , 2010, Animal Behaviour.

[29]  Adam Powell,et al.  Late Pleistocene Demography and the Appearance of Modern Human Behavior , 2009, Science.

[30]  M. J. O’Brien,et al.  Risk of Resource Failure and Toolkit Variation in Small-Scale Farmers and Herders , 2012, PloS one.

[31]  Krist Vaesen,et al.  Cumulative Cultural Evolution and Demography , 2012, PloS one.

[32]  K. Laland,et al.  Identification of the Social and Cognitive Processes Underlying Human Cumulative Culture , 2012, Science.

[33]  J. Lind,et al.  The Number of Cultural Traits Is Correlated with Female Group Size but Not with Male Group Size in Chimpanzee Communities , 2010, PloS one.

[34]  Kevin N Laland,et al.  Culture evolves , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[35]  Yutaka Kobayashi,et al.  Innovativeness, population size and cumulative cultural evolution. , 2012, Theoretical population biology.

[36]  Steven L. Kuhn,et al.  Modeling Effects of Local Extinctions on Culture Change and Diversity in the Paleolithic , 2010, PloS one.

[37]  Carel P. van Schaik,et al.  Explaining brain size variation: from social to cultural brain , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[38]  J. Henrich,et al.  The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[39]  Mark Collard,et al.  What drives the evolution of hunter–gatherer subsistence technology? A reanalysis of the risk hypothesis with data from the Pacific Northwest , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[40]  J. Henrich,et al.  The evolution of cultural evolution , 2003 .

[41]  Laurent Lehmann,et al.  On the number of independent cultural traits carried by individuals and populations , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[42]  Laurent Lehmann,et al.  Rates of cultural change and patterns of cultural accumulation in stochastic models of social transmission. , 2011, Theoretical population biology.

[43]  C. Caldwell,et al.  Human cumulative culture in the laboratory: Effects of (micro) population size , 2010, Learning & behavior.

[44]  Stephen Shennan,et al.  Innovation in cultural systems : contributions from evolutionary anthropology , 2009 .