Constraints on the Learning of Spatial Terms: A Computational Investigation
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Michael R. Waldmann,et al. Causal models and the acquisition of category structure , 1995 .
[2] Leonard Talmy,et al. Semantic Structures in English and Atsugewi , 1972 .
[3] M. Bowerman,et al. Learning to express motion events in English and Korean: The influence of language-specific lexicalization patterns , 1991, Cognition.
[4] Michael Gasser,et al. Learning to Talk About the Properties of Objects: A Network Model of the Development of Dimensions , 1997 .
[5] E. Markman,et al. Sixteen- and 24-month-olds' use of mutual exclusivity as a default assumption in second-label learning , 1994 .
[6] Eve V. Clark,et al. The principle of contrast: A constraint on language acquisition. , 1987 .
[7] P. C. Bomba,et al. The development of orientation categories between 2 and 4 months of age. , 1984, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[8] S. Ullman. Visual routines , 1984, Cognition.
[9] Claudia Brugman. The Use of Body-part Terms as Locatives in Chalcatongo Mixtec , 1983 .
[10] H. H. Clark. SPACE, TIME, SEMANTICS, AND THE CHILD , 1973 .
[11] Fernand Gabriel Renier,et al. Dutch Dictionary: Dutch-English, English-Dutch , 1982 .
[12] G D Logan,et al. Distance and distraction effects in the apprehension of spatial relations. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[13] A. Glenberg,et al. What memory is for: Creating meaning in the service of action , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[14] James L. McClelland,et al. Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 1: foundations , 1986 .
[15] Elizabeth Bates,et al. Books Recieved , 2022, CL.
[16] B. Cohen,et al. Coding of information about rapid eye movements in the pontine reticular formation of alert monkeys , 1976, Brain Research.
[17] J. Elman. Learning and development in neural networks: the importance of starting small , 1993, Cognition.
[18] L. Hedges,et al. Categories and particulars: prototype effects in estimating spatial location. , 1991, Psychological review.
[19] Soonja Choi,et al. Verbs, particles, and spatial semantics: Learning to talk about spatial actions in typologically different languages , 1995 .
[20] M. Bowerman. Learning how to structure space for language: A crosslinguistic perspective , 1996 .
[21] Elie Bienenstock,et al. Neural Networks and the Bias/Variance Dilemma , 1992, Neural Computation.
[22] Eric Mjolsness,et al. A center-of-mass computation describes the precision of random dot displacement discrimination , 1992, Vision Research.
[23] M. Barrett,et al. Lexical development and overextension in child language , 1978, Journal of Child Language.
[24] Steven Pinker,et al. Language learnability and language development , 1985 .
[25] G. Lakoff,et al. Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal about the Mind , 1988 .
[26] J. Mandler. How to build a baby: II. Conceptual primitives. , 1992, Psychological review.
[27] D. Hubel,et al. Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex , 1962, The Journal of physiology.
[28] S. Levinson,et al. LANGUAGE AND SPACE , 1996 .
[29] L. Barsalou,et al. Mundane creativity in perceptual symbol systems , 1997 .
[30] Ellen M. Markman,et al. Categorization and Naming in Children: Problems of Induction , 1989 .
[31] E. Markman,et al. Children's use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words , 1988, Cognitive Psychology.
[32] R. Brown,et al. A First Language , 1973 .
[33] Yann LeCun,et al. Generalization and network design strategies , 1989 .
[34] E. Markman,et al. The mutual exclusivity bias in children's word learning: By W. E. Merriman and L. L. Bowman. In Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 54, Serial No. 220, 1989☆ , 1991 .
[35] Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al. Learning internal representations by error propagation , 1986 .
[36] V. J. Wilson,et al. Body position with respect to the head or body position in space is coded by lumbar interneurons. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.
[37] Anders Krogh,et al. Introduction to the theory of neural computation , 1994, The advanced book program.
[38] D. Medin,et al. Linear separability in classification learning. , 1981 .
[39] Thomas G. Dietterich,et al. Readings in Machine Learning , 1991 .
[40] Gordon D. Logan,et al. A computational analysis of the apprehension of spatial relations , 1996 .
[41] T. Regier. A model of the human capacity for categorizing spatial relations , 1995 .
[42] L. Elbers,et al. A tip-of-the-tongue experience at age two? , 1985, Journal of Child Language.
[43] J. Freyd,et al. Representing statics as forces in equilibrium. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[44] Ray Jackendoff. Semantics and Cognition , 1983 .
[45] Ad W. Smitsman,et al. Infants' Perception of Dynamic Relations Between Objects: Passing Through or Support? , 1995 .
[46] A. P. Georgopoulos,et al. Neuronal population coding of movement direction. , 1986, Science.
[47] Jeffrey Mark Siskind,et al. Naive physics, event perception, lexical semantics, and language acquisition , 1992 .
[48] G. Miller,et al. Language and Perception , 1976 .
[49] Amy Needham,et al. Intuitions about support in 4.5-month-old infants , 1993, Cognition.
[50] George Cybenko,et al. Approximation by superpositions of a sigmoidal function , 1992, Math. Control. Signals Syst..
[51] S. Pinker,et al. On language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition , 1988, Cognition.
[52] B. MacWhinney,et al. The Child Language Data Exchange System: an update , 1990, Journal of Child Language.
[53] Mark A. Fanty,et al. Computing with structured neural networks , 1988, Computer.
[54] Terry Regier,et al. The Human Semantic Potential: Spatial Language and Constrained Connectionism , 1996 .
[55] F. Moore. Cognitive development and the acquisition of language , 1973 .
[56] C. Mervis,et al. Two-year-olds readily learn multiple labels for the same basic-level category. , 1994, Child development.
[57] D. Hubel,et al. Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat's striate cortex , 1959, The Journal of physiology.
[58] Melissa Bowerman,et al. How do children avoid constructing an overly general grammar in the absence of feedback about what is not a sentence , 1983 .
[59] D. Slobin,et al. The development of locative expressions in English, Italian, Serbo-Croatian and Turkish , 1977, Journal of Child Language.
[60] Steven Pinker,et al. Lexical and Conceptual Semantics , 1991 .
[61] James L. McClelland,et al. On learning the past-tenses of English verbs: implicit rules or parallel distributed processing , 1986 .
[62] B. Fischhoff,et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory , 1980 .
[63] V. Marchman,et al. U-shaped learning and frequency effects in a multi-layered perception: Implications for child language acquisition , 1991, Cognition.
[64] D. Massaro. Some criticisms of connectionist models of human performance , 1988 .
[65] M. Tarr,et al. Spatial language and spatial representation , 1995, Cognition.
[66] Jeffrey L. Elman,et al. Finding Structure in Time , 1990, Cogn. Sci..
[67] S. Pinker. Learnability and Cognition: The Acquisition of Argument Structure , 1989 .