The operant and the classical in conditioned orientation of Drosophila melanogaster at the flight simulator.

Ever since learning and memory have been studied experimentally, the relationship between operant and classical conditioning has been controversial. Operant conditioning is any form of conditioning that essentially depends on the animal's behavior. It relies on operant behavior. A motor output is called operant if it controls a sensory variable. The Drosophila flight simulator, in which the relevant behavior is a single motor variable (yaw torque), fully separates the operant and classical components of a complex conditioning task. In this paradigm a tethered fly learns, operantly or classically, to prefer and avoid certain flight orientations in relation to the surrounding panorama. Yaw torque is recorded and, in the operant mode, controls the panorama. Using a yoked control, we show that classical pattern learning necessitates more extensive training than operant pattern learning. We compare in detail the microstructure of yaw torque after classical and operant training but find no evidence for acquired behavioral traits after operant conditioning that might explain this difference. We therefore conclude that the operant behavior has a facilitating effect on the classical training. In addition, we show that an operantly learned stimulus is successfully transferred from the behavior of the training to a different behavior. This result unequivocally demonstrates that during operant conditioning classical associations can be formed.

[1]  B. Skinner Two Types of Conditioned Reflex and a Pseudo Type , 1935 .

[2]  J. Konorski,et al.  On Two Types of Conditioned Reflex , 1937 .

[3]  B. Skinner Two Types of Conditioned Reflex: A Reply to Konorski and Miller , 1937 .

[4]  D. Whitteridge Lectures on Conditioned Reflexes , 1942, Nature.

[5]  D. Hebb The distinction between classical and instrumental. , 1956, Canadian journal of psychology.

[6]  C. Burt THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING , 1958 .

[7]  G. Horridge,et al.  Learning of Leg Position by Headless Insects , 1962, Nature.

[8]  R. Rescorla,et al.  Two-process learning theory: Relationships between Pavlovian conditioning and instrumental learning. , 1967, Psychological review.

[9]  M. A. Trapold,et al.  Transfer from classical conditioning and extinction to acquisition, extinction, and stimulus generalization of a positively reinforced instrumental response. , 1967, Journal of experimental psychology.

[10]  G. Lawton,et al.  Transfer of training from differential classical to differential instrumental conditioning. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[11]  R. Rescorla A theory of pavlovian conditioning: The effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement , 1972 .

[12]  D. A. Grant,et al.  Eyelid conditioning performance when the mode of reinforcement is changed from classical to instrumental avoidance and vice versa. , 1974, Journal of experimental psychology.

[13]  I. Gormezano,et al.  The pavlovian analysis of instrumental conditioning , 1976, The Pavlovian journal of biological science.

[14]  G. Hoyle,et al.  Mechanisms of simple motor learning , 1979, Trends in Neurosciences.

[15]  E. Kandel,et al.  A cellular mechanism of classical conditioning in Aplysia: activity-dependent amplification of presynaptic facilitation. , 1983, Science.

[16]  E. Kandel,et al.  Classical conditioning and sensitization share aspects of the same molecular cascade in Aplysia. , 1983, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[17]  M. Heisenberg,et al.  Vision in Drosophila , 1984 .

[18]  T. Carew,et al.  Invertebrate learning and memory: from behavior to molecules. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.

[19]  J. Pearce A model for stimulus generalization in Pavlovian conditioning. , 1987, Psychological review.

[20]  T Preat,et al.  Genetic dissection of memory formation in Drosophila melanogaster. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[21]  Richard S. Sutton,et al.  Time-Derivative Models of Pavlovian Reinforcement , 1990 .

[22]  T. Tully Physiology of mutations affecting learning and memory in Drosophila - the missing link between gene product and behavior , 1991, Trends in Neurosciences.

[23]  David C. Palmer,et al.  Learning and Complex Behavior , 1993 .

[24]  M. Hammer An identified neuron mediates the unconditioned stimulus in associative olfactory learning in honeybees , 1993, Nature.

[25]  J W Donahoe,et al.  A selectionist approach to reinforcement. , 1993, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[26]  M. Heisenberg,et al.  The sensory-motor link in motion-dependent flight control of flies. , 1993, Reviews of oculomotor research.

[27]  Reinhard Wolf,et al.  Visual pattern recognition in Drosophila involves retinotopic matching , 1993, Nature.

[28]  J. Pearce Similarity and Discrimination: A Selective Review and a Connectionist Model , 1994 .

[29]  B. Balleine Asymmetrical Interactions between Thirst and Hunger in Pavlovian-Instrumental Transfer , 1994, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[30]  J. Pearce Similarity and discrimination: a selective review and a connectionist model. , 1994, Psychological review.

[31]  T. Préat,et al.  Genetic dissection of consolidated memory in Drosophila , 1994, Cell.

[32]  R. Rescorla Control of instrumental performance by Pavlovian and instrumental stimuli. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[33]  M Heisenberg,et al.  Visual pattern memory without shape recognition. , 1995, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[34]  David L. Glanzman,et al.  The cellular basis of classical conditioning in Aplysia californica — it's less simple than you think , 1995, Trends in Neurosciences.

[35]  M Heisenberg,et al.  Behavioral analysis of Drosophila landmark learning in the flight simulator. , 1995, Learning & memory.

[36]  M. Heisenberg Pattern recognition in insects , 1995, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[37]  Ralph R. Miller,et al.  Biological Significance as a Determinant of Cue Competition , 1996 .

[38]  M. Heisenberg,et al.  Conditioned visual flight orientation in Drosophila: dependence on age, practice, and diet. , 1996, Learning & memory.

[39]  Björn Brembs Classical and operant conditioning in Drosophila at the flight simulator , 1996 .

[40]  R. Menzel,et al.  Learning and memory in honeybees: from behavior to neural substrates. , 1996, Annual review of neuroscience.

[41]  Jonathan R. Wolpaw,et al.  The complex structure of a simple memory , 1997, Trends in Neurosciences.

[42]  J. Donahoe CHAPTER 18. SELECTION NETWORKS: SIMULATION OF PLASTICITY THROUGH REINFORCEMENT LEARNING , 1997 .

[43]  M Heisenberg,et al.  Visual space from visual motion: turn integration in tethered flying Drosophila. , 1997, Learning & memory.

[44]  D. A. Baxter,et al.  Contingent-Dependent Enhancement of Rhythmic Motor Patterns: AnIn Vitro Analog of Operant Conditioning , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  A Guo,et al.  Association of visual objects and olfactory cues in Drosophila. , 1997, Learning & memory.

[46]  A. Guo,et al.  Memory consolidation in Drosophila operant visual learning. , 1997, Learning & memory.

[47]  J. Donahoe,et al.  The S-R issue: its status in behavior analysis and in Donahoe and Palmer's learning and complex behavior. , 1997, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[48]  R. F. Thompson,et al.  Inhibitory cerebello-olivary projections and blocking effect in classical conditioning. , 1998, Science.

[49]  M Heisenberg,et al.  Drosophila mushroom bodies are dispensable for visual, tactile, and motor learning. , 1998, Learning & memory.

[50]  M. Fanselow,et al.  Pavlovian Conditioning, Negative Feedback, and Blocking: Mechanisms that Regulate Association Formation , 1998, Neuron.

[51]  A. Guo,et al.  Temporary Amnesia Induced by Cold Anesthesia and Hypoxia in Drosophila , 1998, Physiology & Behavior.

[52]  M. Heisenberg,et al.  The memory template in Drosophila pattern vision at the flight simulator , 1999, Vision Research.

[53]  Li Liu,et al.  Context generalization in Drosophila visual learning requires the mushroom bodies , 1999, Nature.

[54]  K. Lukowiak,et al.  Neural Changes after Operant Conditioning of the Aerial Respiratory Behavior in Lymnaea stagnalis , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.