Common and distinct neural networks for false-belief reasoning and inhibitory control

Ample behavioral evidence has shown that the ability to attribute false beliefs as part of a Theory of Mind (ToM) and the ability to inhibit a prepotent response are strongly correlated in both children and adults. Frequently reported areas associated with both processes are the right temporo-parietal junction (TPJ) and the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC). Nevertheless, the exact nature of the relationship between belief-reasoning and inhibitory control at the neural level remains unclear. A functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study was conducted to investigate the neural correlates of belief-reasoning and inhibitory control in a within-subjects design using virtually identical visual stimuli. A false-belief task was used to investigate belief-attribution. The neural correlates of response inhibition were measured using a Go/No-Go task. Besides distinct activation for belief-reasoning and inhibitory control, the results also showed a substantial overlap for both processes in the right superior dorsal MPFC, the right TPJ, the dorsal part of the left TPJ, and lateral prefrontal areas. These findings suggest that the previously described behavioral link between belief attribution and inhibitory control may be explained by a common recruitment of brain areas related to domain-general cognitive processes. Also, the results indicate that neither the right TPJ nor MPFC is specific to the attribution of false beliefs.

[1]  Bernard Mazoyer,et al.  Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: Phonology, semantics, and sentence processing , 2006, NeuroImage.

[2]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  A Developmental Functional MRI Study of Prefrontal Activation during Performance of a Go-No-Go Task , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  Jason P. Mitchell Activity in right temporo-parietal junction is not selective for theory-of-mind. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[4]  T. Goldberg,et al.  Brain regions underlying response inhibition and interference monitoring and suppression , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.

[5]  Marcel Brass,et al.  Control of shared representations relies on key processes involved in mental state attribution , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[6]  Emma Flynn,et al.  The Role of Inhibitory Control in False Belief Understanding , 2007 .

[7]  J Townsend,et al.  Impairment in shifting attention in autistic and cerebellar patients. , 1994, Behavioral neuroscience.

[8]  G. Glover,et al.  Error‐related brain activation during a Go/NoGo response inhibition task , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[9]  M. Corbetta,et al.  The Reorienting System of the Human Brain: From Environment to Theory of Mind , 2008, Neuron.

[10]  M. Kronbichler,et al.  Thinking of mental and other representations: The roles of left and right temporo-parietal junction , 2006, Social neuroscience.

[11]  J. Pekar,et al.  fMRI evidence that the neural basis of response inhibition is task-dependent. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[12]  J. Perner,et al.  Development of theory of mind and executive control , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[13]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind” ? , 1985, Cognition.

[14]  A. Aron The Neural Basis of Inhibition in Cognitive Control , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[15]  Beate Sodian,et al.  True- and false-belief reasoning in children and adults: An event-related potential study of theory of mind , 2011, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[16]  Rolf Verleger,et al.  P3b: Towards some decision about memory , 2008, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[17]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception , 1983, Cognition.

[18]  Stephen Lawrie,et al.  Functional Specialization within Rostral Prefrontal Cortex (Area 10): A Meta-analysis , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  Tor D. Wager,et al.  Common and unique components of response inhibition revealed by fMRI , 2005, NeuroImage.

[20]  Josef Perner,et al.  Training transfer between card sorting and false belief understanding: helping children apply conflicting descriptions. , 2003, Child development.

[21]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Prefrontal‐subcortical dissociations underlying inhibitory control revealed by event‐related fMRI , 2004, The European journal of neuroscience.

[22]  K. Zilles,et al.  Mind Reading: Neural Mechanisms of Theory of Mind and Self-Perspective , 2001, NeuroImage.

[23]  Dana Samson,et al.  Left temporoparietal junction is necessary for representing someone else's belief , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[24]  C. Frith,et al.  Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[25]  Emery N. Brown,et al.  Distinct Regions of Right Temporo-Parietal Junction Are Selective for Theory of Mind and Exogenous Attention , 2009, PloS one.

[26]  Beate Sodian,et al.  Neural correlates of true and false belief reasoning , 2007, NeuroImage.

[27]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  Reading minds versus following rules: Dissociating theory of mind and executive control in the brain , 2006, Social neuroscience.

[28]  J. Polich Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[29]  Lindsey J. Powell,et al.  It's the Thought That Counts , 2006, Psychological science.

[30]  J. Decety,et al.  The Role of the Right Temporoparietal Junction in Social Interaction: How Low-Level Computational Processes Contribute to Meta-Cognition , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[31]  U. Frith,et al.  The impact of extensive medial frontal lobe damage on 'Theory of Mind' and cognition. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[32]  Hiroshi Fukuda,et al.  The human prefrontal and parietal association cortices are involved in NO-GO performances—an event-related fMRI study , 2000, NeuroImage.

[33]  Uta Frith,et al.  Theory of mind , 2001, Current Biology.

[34]  Andrew B. Leber,et al.  Coordination of Voluntary and Stimulus-Driven Attentional Control in Human Cortex , 2005, Psychological science.

[35]  J. Pekar,et al.  Meta-analysis of Go/No-go tasks demonstrating that fMRI activation associated with response inhibition is task-dependent , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  K. Kiehl,et al.  Event‐related fMRI study of response inhibition , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[37]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Imaging Cognition II: An Empirical Review of 275 PET and fMRI Studies , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[38]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Dissociable mechanisms of cognitive control in prefrontal and premotor cortex. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  K. Berman,et al.  Meta‐analysis of neuroimaging studies of the Wisconsin Card‐Sorting task and component processes , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[40]  C. Frith,et al.  Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[41]  Stephanie M. Carlson,et al.  The Development of Executive Functioning and Theory of Mind , 2006, Psychological science.

[42]  Vince D. Calhoun,et al.  Functional neural networks underlying response inhibition in adolescents and adults , 2007, Behavioural Brain Research.

[43]  M. Corbetta,et al.  An Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of Voluntary and Stimulus-Driven Orienting of Attention , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  R. E Passingham,et al.  Inferring false beliefs from the actions of oneself and others: an fMRI study , 2004, NeuroImage.

[45]  P. Ellen Grant,et al.  Developmental neural networks in children performing a Categorical N-Back Task , 2006, NeuroImage.

[46]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Frontal and Temporo-Parietal Lobe Contributions to Theory of Mind: Neuropsychological Evidence from a False-Belief Task with Reduced Language and Executive Demands , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[47]  Kevin Murphy,et al.  Individual differences in the functional neuroanatomy of inhibitory control , 2006, Brain Research.

[48]  Stephanie M. Carlson,et al.  How specific is the relation between executive function and theory of mind? Contributions of inhibitory control and working memory , 2002 .

[49]  Jesper Andersson,et al.  Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic , 2005, NeuroImage.

[50]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[51]  Kevin Murphy,et al.  Beyond common resources: the cortical basis for resolving task interference , 2004, NeuroImage.

[52]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension , 1995, Cognition.

[53]  Josef Perner,et al.  Temporo-parietal Junction Activity in Theory-of-Mind Tasks: Falseness, Beliefs, or Attention , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[54]  D. Poeppel,et al.  Dorsal and ventral streams: a framework for understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language , 2004, Cognition.

[55]  Alan M. Leslie,et al.  Belief-desire reasoning as a process of selection , 2005, Cognitive Psychology.

[56]  B. Pennington,et al.  Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: relationship to theory of mind. , 1991, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[57]  Josef Perner,et al.  Do visual perspective tasks need theory of mind? , 2006, NeuroImage.

[58]  Aaron C. Koralek,et al.  Two Takes on the Social Brain: A Comparison of Theory of Mind Tasks , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[59]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[60]  C. Frith,et al.  Performance-related activity in medial rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) during low-demand tasks. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[61]  H. Wellman,et al.  Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. , 2001, Child development.

[62]  L. Moses,et al.  Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind. , 2001, Child development.