Learning motion: Human vs. optimal Bayesian learner

We used the optimal perceptual learning paradigm (Eckstein, Abbey, Pham, & Shimozaki, 2004) to investigate the dynamics of human rapid learning processes in motion discrimination tasks and compare it to an optimal Bayesian learner. This paradigm consists of blocks of few trials defined by a set of target attributes, and it has been shown its ability to detect learning effects appearing as soon as after the first trial. In the present task a sequence consisting of four patches containing random-dot patterns is presented at four separate locations equidistant from a fixation point. On each trial, the random dots in three patches moved with a mean speed and the fourth, target patch, could move either with slower or faster mean speed. Observers' task was to indicate what speed, faster or slower, was present in the display. The mean direction of the target patch was kept invariant along a block of trials. Observers learned the target relevant motion direction through indirect feedback, leading to an improvement in speed identification performance ranging from 15% to 30% which is greater than previously studied contrast defined targets and faces. However, comparison to an ideal learner revealed incomplete or partial learning for the motion task which was lower than previously measured for contrast defined targets and faces. A sub-optimal model that included inefficiencies in the updating of motion direction weights due to memory effects could account for the human learning. Finally, the similarity of the rapid learning effect observed here for motion perception with that found for contrast defined targets for localization and identification tasks could be suggesting a general strategy for learning in the human visual system and some common limitations such as memory.

[1]  L. Vaina,et al.  Learning to ignore: psychophysics and computational modeling of fast learning of direction in noisy motion stimuli. , 1995, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[2]  David C. Knill,et al.  Object classification for human and ideal observers , 1995, Vision Research.

[3]  B. Picinbono,et al.  Detection and contrast , 1986 .

[4]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Direction-specific improvement in motion discrimination , 1987, Vision Research.

[5]  L. Maloney,et al.  Perceptual organization and neural computation. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[6]  Robert A Jacobs,et al.  Adaptive precision pooling of model neuron activities predicts the efficiency of human visual learning. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[7]  R. Jacobs,et al.  Perceptual learning for a pattern discrimination task , 2000, Vision Research.

[8]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .

[9]  F A Wichmann,et al.  Ning for Helpful Comments and Suggestions. This Paper Benefited Con- Siderably from Conscientious Peer Review, and We Thank Our Reviewers the Psychometric Function: I. Fitting, Sampling, and Goodness of Fit , 2001 .

[10]  Miguel P. Eckstein,et al.  Detection and contrast discrimination of moving signals in uncorrelated Gaussian noise , 1996, Medical Imaging.

[11]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[12]  Daniel Kersten,et al.  Bayesian models of object perception , 2003, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[13]  B L Beard,et al.  Detection in fixed and random noise in foveal and parafoveal vision explained by template learning. , 1999, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[14]  W. Geisler Ideal Observer Analysis , 2002 .

[15]  G. Grisetti,et al.  Further Reading , 1984, IEEE Spectrum.

[16]  David C. Knill,et al.  Object Classification for Human and Ideal , 1995 .

[17]  S. McKee,et al.  Improvement in vernier acuity with practice , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.

[18]  M. Eckstein,et al.  Perceptual learning through optimization of attentional weighting: human versus optimal Bayesian learner. , 2004, Journal of vision.

[19]  Matthew F. Peterson,et al.  Statistical decision theory to relate neurons to behavior in the study of covert visual attention , 2009, Vision Research.

[20]  O. Braddick,et al.  Speed and direction of locally-paired dot patterns , 2000, Vision Research.

[21]  A. Karni,et al.  The time course of learning a visual skill , 1993, Nature.

[22]  Zili Liu,et al.  Dissociating stimulus information from internal representation—a case study in object recognition , 1999, Vision Research.

[23]  Gordon E. Legge,et al.  The viewpoint complexity of an object-recognition task , 1998, Vision Research.

[24]  L. Chalupa,et al.  The visual neurosciences , 2004 .

[25]  C D Frith,et al.  Modulating irrelevant motion perception by varying attentional load in an unrelated task. , 1997, Science.

[26]  Miguel P Eckstein,et al.  Contrast and stimulus information effects in rapid learning of a visual task. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[27]  R. Sekuler,et al.  A specific and enduring improvement in visual motion discrimination. , 1982, Science.

[28]  F. Scharnowski,et al.  Long-lasting modulation of feature integration by transcranial magnetic stimulation. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[29]  Craig K. Abbey,et al.  The surprisingly high human efficiency at learning to recognize faces , 2009, Vision Research.

[30]  D. Levi,et al.  Perceptual learning in vernier acuity: What is learned? , 1995, Vision Research.

[31]  Jacobs Robert,et al.  Adaptive precision pooling of model neuron activities predicts efficiency of human visual learning , 2009 .

[32]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Perceptual learning of spatial localization: specificity for orientation, position, and context. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  Ione Fine,et al.  Interactions between contrast, coherence and directional tuning , 2004 .

[34]  R A Kinchla,et al.  Precue effects in visual search: Data or resource limited? , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[35]  Richard F Murray,et al.  A linear cue combination framework for understanding selective attention. , 2003, Journal of vision.

[36]  S. Edelman,et al.  Long-term learning in vernier acuity: Effects of stimulus orientation, range and of feedback , 1993, Vision Research.

[37]  H Barlow,et al.  Correspondence Noise and Signal Pooling in the Detection of Coherent Visual Motion , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[39]  Robert Sekuler,et al.  Coherent global motion percepts from stochastic local motions , 1984, Vision Research.

[40]  E. Courchesne,et al.  Attentional Activation of the Cerebellum Independent of Motor Involvement , 1997, Science.

[41]  A. Hurlbert,et al.  Visual perception: Learning to see through noise , 2000, Current Biology.

[42]  W. Richards,et al.  Perception as Bayesian Inference , 2008 .

[43]  Pascal Mamassian,et al.  The efficiency of speed discrimination for coherent and transparent motion , 2003, Vision Research.

[44]  G. Orban,et al.  Human velocity and direction discrimination measured with random dot patterns , 1988, Vision Research.

[45]  Miguel P Eckstein,et al.  Attentional Cues in Real Scenes, Saccadic Targeting, and Bayesian Priors , 2005, Psychological science.

[46]  Nestor Matthews,et al.  Task-specific perceptual learning on speed and direction discrimination , 2003, Vision Research.

[47]  N. Qian,et al.  Perceptual learning on orientation and direction discrimination , 1999, Vision Research.

[48]  Miguel P Eckstein,et al.  The footprints of visual attention in the Posner cueing paradigm revealed by classification images. , 2002, Journal of vision.

[49]  Robert L. Goldstone,et al.  Definition , 1960, A Philosopher Looks at Sport.

[50]  A. B. Sekuler,et al.  Signal but not noise changes with perceptual learning , 1999, Nature.

[51]  Robert A. Jacobs,et al.  Comparing perceptual learning across tasks: A review , 2002 .

[52]  Z L Lu,et al.  Perceptual learning reflects external noise filtering and internal noise reduction through channel reweighting. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[53]  E. Gibson,et al.  An Ecological Approach to Perceptual Learning and Development , 2000 .

[54]  Christopher W. Tyler,et al.  Spatial summation of face information , 2000, Electronic Imaging.

[55]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[56]  H. Barlow The absolute efficiency of perceptual decisions. , 1980, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[57]  Jason M. Gold Dynamic classification images reveal the effects of perceptual learning in a hyperacuity task , 2010 .

[58]  L. Vaina,et al.  Neural systems underlying learning and representation of global motion. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[59]  Miguel P Eckstein,et al.  Comparison of two weighted integration models for the cueing task: linear and likelihood. , 2003, Journal of vision.

[60]  D. Kersten Spatial summation in visual noise , 1984, Vision Research.