BREAKING THE LINGUISTIC CODE: CURRENT ISSUES IN EARLY LANGUAGE LEARNING
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] R. Schreuder,et al. From concepts to lexical items , 1992, Cognition.
[2] S Goldin-Meadow,et al. Gestural communication in deaf children: the effects and noneffects of parental input on early language development. , 1984, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.
[3] A. Michotte. The perception of causality , 1963 .
[4] Terry Kit-fong Au,et al. Input Vs Constraints: Early Word Acquisition in Korean and English , 1994 .
[5] J. Morgan. A Rhythmic Bias in Preverbal Speech Segmentation , 1996 .
[6] D. Scott,et al. Duration as a cue to the perception of a phrase boundary. , 1982, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[7] M. Tomasello. Do young children have adult syntactic competence? , 2000, Cognition.
[8] M. Maratsos. Are actions to verbs as objects are to nouns? On the differential semantic bases of form, class, category , 1990 .
[9] G. Csibra,et al. Goal attribution without agency cues: the perception of ‘pure reason’ in infancy , 1999, Cognition.
[10] Elissa L. Newport,et al. Maturational Constraints on Language Learning , 1990, Cogn. Sci..
[11] James L. Morgan,et al. Signal to Syntax: An Overview , 1996 .
[12] Antal van den Bosch,et al. A Connectionist Model for Bootstrap Learning of Syllabic Structure. , 1998 .
[13] A. Friederici,et al. Phonotactic knowledge of word boundaries and its use in infant speech perception , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.
[14] H. Gleitman,et al. Human simulations of vocabulary learning , 1999, Cognition.
[15] P. Jusczyk,et al. Infants′ Detection of the Sound Patterns of Words in Fluent Speech , 1995, Cognitive Psychology.
[16] Bezalel Elan Dresher,et al. Charting the Learning Path: Cues to Parameter Setting , 1999, Linguistic Inquiry.
[17] Letitia R. Naigles,et al. Motion-Verb Generalizations in English and Spanish: Influences of Language and Syntax , 1998 .
[18] Renée Baillargeon,et al. The development of calibration-based reasoning about collision events in young infants , 1998, Cognition.
[19] C. Fisher. Structural Limits on Verb Mapping: The Role of Analogy in Children's Interpretations of Sentences , 1996, Cognitive Psychology.
[20] Anne Cutler,et al. Exploiting prosodic probabilities in speech segmentation , 1991 .
[21] C. Fisher,et al. Abstraction and Specificity in Preschoolers' Representations of Novel Spoken Words , 2001 .
[22] Noam Chomsky,et al. The Minimalist Program , 1992 .
[23] P. Jusczyk,et al. Infants' preference for the predominant stress patterns of English words. , 1993, Child development.
[24] S. Waxman,et al. The development of a linkage between count nouns and object categories: evidence from fifteen- to twenty-one-month-old infants. , 1993, Child development.
[25] J. Lucy,et al. Language acquisition and conceptual development: Grammatical categories and the development of classification preferences: a comparative approach , 2001 .
[26] Patrice D. Tremoulet,et al. Perceptual causality and animacy , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[27] Noam Chomsky,et al. The Sound Pattern of English , 1968 .
[28] F. Guenther,et al. The perceptual magnet effect as an emergent property of neural map formation. , 1996, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[29] D. Gentner,et al. Language acquisition and conceptual development: Individuation, relativity, and early word learning , 2001 .
[30] Penelope Brown. Learning to talk about motion UP and DOWN in Tzeltal: Is there a language-specific bias for verb learning? , 2001 .
[31] B. L. Whorf. Language, Thought, and Reality: Selected Writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf , 1956 .
[32] P. Jusczyk,et al. Clauses are perceptual units for young infants , 1987, Cognition.
[33] F. Heider,et al. An experimental study of apparent behavior , 1944 .
[34] J. S. Johnson,et al. Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language , 1989, Cognitive Psychology.
[35] Jacques Mehler,et al. How do 4-day-old infants categorize multisyllabic utterances? , 1993 .
[36] Mark A. Pitt,et al. Phonological and Morphological Influences in the Syllabification of Spoken Words , 1999 .
[37] Nick Chater,et al. Distributional Information: A Powerful Cue for Acquiring Syntactic Categories , 1998, Cogn. Sci..
[38] Dan I. Slobin,et al. Language acquisition and conceptual development: Form–function relations: how do children find out what they are? , 2001 .
[39] Scott P. Johnson,et al. Synchronous change and perception of object unity: evidence from adults and infants , 1999, Cognition.
[40] Nan Bernstein Ratner,et al. Durational cues which mark clause boundaries in mother–child speech , 1986 .
[41] Janet B. Pierrehumbert,et al. Phonological and phonetic representation , 1990 .
[42] How mothers teach basic-level and situation-restricted count nouns. , 1994 .
[43] Elissa L Newport,et al. Structural packaging in the input to language learning: Contributions of prosodic and morphological marking of phrases to the acquisition of language , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.
[44] Dare A. Baldwin,et al. Early referential understanding: Infants' ability to recognize referential acts for what they are. , 1993 .
[45] Charles J. Fillmore,et al. THE CASE FOR CASE. , 1967 .
[46] Anne Cutler,et al. The syllable's differing role in the segmentation of French and English. , 1986 .
[47] Dare A. Baldwin,et al. Infants' ability to draw inferences about nonobvious object properties: evidence from exploratory play. , 1993, Child development.
[48] M. Bowerman. Learning how to structure space for language: A crosslinguistic perspective , 1996 .
[49] M. H. Kelly,et al. Phonological biases in grammatical category shifts , 1988 .
[50] J. Werker,et al. Infants listen for more phonetic detail in speech perception than in word-learning tasks , 1997, Nature.
[51] C. Fillmore. The case for case reopened , 1977 .
[52] Elissa L. Newport,et al. The role of constituent structure in the induction of an artificial language , 1981 .
[53] Noam Chomsky,et al. वाक्यविन्यास का सैद्धान्तिक पक्ष = Aspects of the theory of syntax , 1965 .
[54] D. Duez. Acoustic correlates of subjective pauses , 1993 .
[55] D B Pisoni,et al. Some effects of laboratory training on identification and discrimination of voicing contrasts in stop consonants. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[56] Willard Van Orman Quine,et al. Word and Object , 1960 .
[57] P. Jusczyk,et al. Infants' sensitivity to phonotactic patterns in the native language. , 1994 .
[58] Letitia R. Naigles,et al. Children use syntax to learn verb meanings , 1990, Journal of Child Language.
[59] J. Chafetz,et al. Verb-based versus class-based accounts of actionality effects in children's comprehension of passives , 1990, Cognition.
[60] R. Aslin,et al. Three- and four-year-olds’ perceptual confusions for spoken words , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.
[61] Jeffrey Gruber,et al. Look and See , 1967 .
[62] M. Braine. What sort of innate structure is needed to “bootstrap” into syntax? , 1992, Cognition.
[63] Kenneth Ward Church,et al. Phonological parsing and lexical retrieval , 1987, Cognition.
[64] Susan M. Garnsey,et al. Semantic Influences On Parsing: Use of Thematic Role Information in Syntactic Ambiguity Resolution , 1994 .
[65] A. Meltzoff. Understanding the Intentions of Others: Re-Enactment of Intended Acts by 18-Month-Old Children. , 1995, Developmental psychology.
[66] M. Tomasello,et al. Joint attention and early language. , 1986, Child development.
[67] J. Werker,et al. Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life , 1984 .
[68] L. Bloom. Language Development: Form and Function in Emerging Grammars , 1970 .
[69] C. Pallier,et al. A limit on behavioral plasticity in speech perception , 1997, Cognition.
[70] J. D. Miller,et al. Speech perception by the chinchilla: voiced-voiceless distinction in alveolar plosive consonants , 1975, Science.
[71] Dare A. Baldwin,et al. The Ontogeny of Social Information Gathering , 1996 .
[72] M. Tomasello. First Verbs: A Case Study of Early Grammatical Development , 1994 .
[73] Ray Jackendoff. Semantics and Cognition , 1983 .
[74] Michael P Maratsos. The acquisition of grammar , 1983 .
[75] Cynthia Fisher,et al. On the semantic content of subcategorization frames , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.
[76] E. Spelke,et al. Ontological categories guide young children's inductions of word meaning: Object terms and substance terms , 1991, Cognition.
[77] Cheryl M. Beach,et al. The interpretation of prosodic patterns at points of syntactic structure ambiguity: Evidence for cue trading relations☆ , 1991 .
[78] A. Meltzoff,et al. Infant vocalizations in response to speech: vocal imitation and developmental change. , 1996, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[79] D. Slobin. From “thought and language” to “thinking for speaking” , 1996 .
[80] John A. Goldsmith,et al. Unsupervised Learning of the Morphology of a Natural Language , 2001, CL.
[81] E. Spelke,et al. Perception of partly occluded objects in infancy , 1983, Cognitive Psychology.
[82] P K Kuhl,et al. Enhanced discriminability at the phonetic boundaries for the voicing feature in macaques , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.
[83] S. Trehub,et al. Infants’ perception of musical patterns , 1987, Perception & psychophysics.
[84] J. Mandler. Perceptual and Conceptual Processes in Infancy , 2000 .
[85] Maureen A. Callanan,et al. How Parents Label Objects for Young Children: The Role of Input in the Acquisition of Category Hierarchies. , 1985 .
[86] M. H. Kelly,et al. Using sound to solve syntactic problems: the role of phonology in grammatical category assignments. , 1992, Psychological review.
[87] I. Lehiste. Rhythmic units and syntactic units in production and perception. , 1973, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[88] Sandra R. Waxman,et al. A Cross-Linguistic Examination of the Noun-Category Bias: Its Existence and Specificity in French- and Spanish-Speaking Preschool-Aged Children , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.
[89] Silvia P. Gennari,et al. Knowing versus Naming: Similarity and the Linguistic Categorization of Artifacts , 1999 .
[90] Colin W. Wightman,et al. Segmental durations in the vicinity of prosodic phrase boundaries. , 1992, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[91] Zellig S. Harris,et al. Methods in structural linguistics. , 1952 .
[92] J. Fodor. The Language of Thought , 1980 .
[93] S. Goldin-Meadow,et al. Spontaneous sign systems created by deaf children in two cultures , 1998, Nature.
[94] D. Klatt. Linguistic uses of segmental duration in English: acoustic and perceptual evidence. , 1976, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[95] Peggy Li,et al. Can the situations in which words occur account for cross-linguistic variation in vocabulary composition? , 2000 .
[96] Thierry Nazzi,et al. Language Discrimination by Newborns: Toward an Understanding of the Role of Rhythm , 1998 .
[97] K. Nelson,et al. Nouns in early lexicons: evidence, explanations and implications , 1993, Journal of Child Language.
[98] P. Jusczyk,et al. Sensitivity to discontinuous dependencies in language learners: evidence for limitations in processing space , 1998, Cognition.
[99] A. Fernald,et al. Common themes and cultural variations in Japanese and American mothers' speech to infants. , 1993, Child development.
[100] E. Markman,et al. Children's use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words , 1988, Cognitive Psychology.
[101] Steven Pinker,et al. Language learnability and language development , 1985 .
[102] LouAnn Gerken,et al. Interplay of Function Morphemes and Prosody in Early Language , 1993 .
[103] E. Markman,et al. When it is better to receive than to give: Syntactic and conceptual constraints on vocabulary growth , 1994 .
[104] B. Levin,et al. What to do with theta-roles , 1986 .
[105] B. L. Whorf. The Relation of Habitual Thought and Behavior to Language , 1997 .
[106] C. Fisher,et al. Acoustic cues to grammatical structure in infant-directed speech: cross-linguistic evidence. , 1996, Child development.
[107] J. Werker,et al. Influences on infant speech processing: toward a new synthesis. , 1999, Annual review of psychology.
[108] U. Frauenfelder,et al. The Role of the Syllable in Language Acquisition and Perception , 1981 .
[109] P. Jusczyk,et al. When prosody fails to cue syntactic structure: 9-month-olds' sensitivity to phonological versus syntactic phrases , 1994, Cognition.
[110] C. Snow. Mothers' Speech to Children Learning Language. , 1972 .
[111] J. Werker,et al. Newborn infants’ sensitivity to perceptual cues to lexical and grammatical words , 1999, Cognition.
[112] Patricia M. Clancy,et al. The acquisition of Japanese , 1986 .
[113] Lila R. Gleitman,et al. Why It Is Hard to Label Our Concepts. , 2004 .
[114] W. Cooper,et al. Fundamental frequency contours at syntactic boundaries. , 1977, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[115] Irina A. Sekerina,et al. The kindergarten-path effect: studying on-line sentence processing in young children , 1999, Cognition.
[116] Peter W. Jusczyk,et al. Does sentential prosody help infants organize and remember speech information? , 1994, Cognition.
[117] M. Bowerman,et al. Learning to express motion events in English and Korean: The influence of language-specific lexicalization patterns , 1991, Cognition.
[118] Susan M. Garnsey,et al. The Contributions of Verb Bias and Plausibility to the Comprehension of Temporarily Ambiguous Sentences , 1997 .
[119] Eve V. Clark,et al. The principle of contrast: A constraint on language acquisition. , 1987 .
[120] Paul D. Allopenna,et al. Phonological and acoustic bases for earliest grammatical category assignment: a cross-linguistic perspective , 1998, Journal of Child Language.
[121] R. Clark. The Selection of Syntactic Knowledge , 1992 .
[122] M. R. Manzini. Learnability and Cognition , 1991 .
[123] A. Leslie,et al. Do six-month-old infants perceive causality? , 1987, Cognition.
[124] E. Newport,et al. Computation of Conditional Probability Statistics by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1998 .
[125] A. Ninio. Ostensive definition in vocabulary teaching , 1980, Journal of Child Language.
[126] Elizabeth Bates,et al. A cross-linguistic study of early lexical development , 1995 .
[127] A. Woodward,et al. Perception of acoustic correlates of major phrasal units by young infants , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.
[128] P. Jusczyk,et al. Young infants' retention of information about bisyllabic utterances. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[129] L. Lisker,et al. A Cross-Language Study of Voicing in Initial Stops: Acoustical Measurements , 1964 .
[130] P. Jusczyk,et al. Infants remember the order of words in a spoken sentence , 1996 .
[131] James Paul Gee,et al. Performance structures: A psycholinguistic and linguistic appraisal , 1983, Cognitive Psychology.
[132] Toben H. Mintz,et al. Adjectives really do modify nouns: the incremental and restricted nature of early adjective acquisition , 2002, Cognition.
[133] M. Bowerman. Mapping thematic roles onto syntactic functions: are children helped by innate linking rules? , 1990 .
[134] Catherine E. Snow,et al. The Critical Period for Language Acquisition: Evidence from Second Language Learning. , 1978 .
[135] Peter D. Eimas,et al. Auditory and phonetic coding of the cues for speech: Discrimination of the [r-l] distinction by young infants , 1975 .
[136] Roger S. Brown,et al. Linguistic determinism and the part of speech. , 1957, Journal of abnormal psychology.
[137] L. Gleitman,et al. Language and Experience: Evidence from the Blind Child , 1988 .
[138] Katherine Nelson,et al. Constraints on word learning , 1988 .
[139] D. Gentner,et al. A cross-linguistic study of early word meaning: universal ontology and linguistic influence , 1997, Cognition.
[140] Elisabeth Selkirk,et al. Phonology and Syntax: The Relation between Sound and Structure , 1984 .
[141] M. Kjelgaard,et al. Prosodic Facilitation and Interference in the Resolution of Temporary Syntactic Closure Ambiguity , 1999 .
[142] S. Pinker. The Language Instinct , 1994 .
[143] J. McQueen. Segmentation of Continuous Speech Using Phonotactics , 1998 .
[144] Ray Jackendoff,et al. Multiple subcategorization and the ϑ-criterion: The case of climb , 1985 .
[145] E. Markman,et al. Children's sensitivity to constraints on word meaning: Taxonomic versus thematic relations , 1984, Cognitive Psychology.
[146] F. Ramus,et al. Correlates of linguistic rhythm in the speech signal , 1999, Cognition.
[147] A. Leslie. A theory of agency. , 1995 .
[148] P. Jusczyk,et al. Infants′ Sensitivity to the Sound Patterns of Native Language Words , 1993 .
[149] Sarah Brown-Schmidt,et al. One Frog, Two Frog, Red Frog, Blue Frog: Factors Affecting Children's Syntactic Choices in Production and Comprehension , 2000, Journal of psycholinguistic research.
[150] D. Geoffrey Hall,et al. Acquiring Proper Nouns for Familiar and Unfamiliar Animate Objects: Two-Year-Olds' Word-learning Biases , 1991 .
[151] George Lakoff,et al. Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things , 1987 .
[152] William E. Cooper,et al. Syntax and Speech , 1980 .
[153] A. Woodward. Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach , 1998, Cognition.
[154] P. Eimas. Segmental and syllabic representations in the perception of speech by young infants. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[155] B. Levin. Unaccusativity: At the Syntax-Lexical Semantics Interface , 1994 .
[156] Sandra R. Waxman,et al. Words as Invitations to Form Categories: Evidence from 12- to 13-Month-Old Infants , 1995, Cognitive Psychology.
[157] L. Gleitman. The Structural Sources of Verb Meanings , 2020, Sentence First, Arguments Afterward.
[158] J. Kegl. Creation through contact : Sign language emergence and sign language change in Nicaragua , 1999 .
[159] Elizabeth F. Shipley,et al. Mothers' use of superordinate category terms , 1983, Journal of Child Language.
[160] D. Sperber,et al. Relevance: Communication and Cognition , 1997 .
[161] Robert L. Goldstone,et al. Altering object representations through category learning , 2001, Cognition.
[162] M. R. Brent,et al. Surface cues and robust inference as a basis for the early acquisition of subcategorization frames , 1994 .
[163] Sandra R. Waxman,et al. Assumptions about Word Meaning: Individuation and Basic‐Level Kinds , 1993 .
[164] M. Bowerman. What Shapes Children's Grammars? , 1985 .
[165] S. Waxman,et al. Do words facilitate object categorization in 9-month-old infants? , 1997, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[166] P C Gordon,et al. Lexical and prelexical influences on word segmentation: evidence from priming. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[167] J. Bertoncini,et al. Newborns discriminate the rhythm of multisyllabic stressed words. , 1997 .
[168] R. Baillargeon,et al. Infants' use of featural and experiential information in segregating and individuating objects: a reply to Xu, Carey and Welch (2000) , 2000, Cognition.
[169] Matthew Rispoli,et al. Encounters with Japanese verbs: caregiver sentences and the categorization of transitive and intransitive action verbs , 1989 .
[170] James L. McClelland,et al. Cognitive penetration of the mechanisms of perception: Compensation for coarticulation of lexically restored phonemes , 1988 .
[171] R N Aslin,et al. Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996, Science.
[172] E. Clark,et al. Speaker perspective in language acquisition , 1990 .
[173] T. A. Cartwright,et al. Distributional regularity and phonotactic constraints are useful for segmentation , 1996, Cognition.
[174] P. Kuhl. Learning and representation in speech and language , 1994, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[175] Barbara Landau,et al. Function Morphemes in Young Children's Speech Perception and Production , 1990 .
[176] M. Hauser,et al. Segmentation of the speech stream in a non-human primate: statistical learning in cotton-top tamarins , 2001, Cognition.
[177] Sandra R. Waxman,et al. How Two‐ and Four‐Year‐Old Children Interpret Adjectives and Count Nouns , 1993 .
[178] G. Pullum. The great Eskimo vocabulary hoax, and other irreverent essays on the study of language , 1991 .
[179] S. Oyama. The Sensitive Period and Comprehension of Speech , 1978 .
[180] Twila Tardif,et al. Putting the "Noun Bias" in Context: A Comparison of English and Mandarin. , 1999 .
[181] James L. Morgan,et al. From Simple Input to Complex Grammar , 1986 .
[182] E. Kako,et al. First contact in verb acquisition: defining a role for syntax. , 1993, Child development.
[183] K. Stevens,et al. Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age. , 1992, Science.
[184] B. C. Griffith,et al. The discrimination of speech sounds within and across phoneme boundaries. , 1957, Journal of experimental psychology.
[185] Dedre Gentner,et al. Why Nouns Are Learned before Verbs: Linguistic Relativity Versus Natural Partitioning. Technical Report No. 257. , 1982 .
[186] Barbara Landau,et al. Count nouns, adjectives, and perceptual properties in children's novel word interpretations. , 1992 .
[187] T. A. Cartwright,et al. Syntactic categorization in early language acquisition: formalizing the role of distributional analysis , 1997, Cognition.
[188] Janet F. Werker,et al. Cross-language speech perception: Initial capabilities and developmental change. , 1988 .
[189] R. Baillargeon,et al. 2.5-Month-Old Infants' Reasoning about When Objects Should and Should Not Be Occluded , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.
[190] A. Gopnik,et al. Early acquisition of verbs in Korean: a cross-linguistic study , 1995, Journal of Child Language.
[191] M. H. Kelly,et al. Domain-general abilities applied to domain-specific tasks: Sensitivity to probabilities in perception, cognition, and language☆ , 1994 .
[192] Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al. Language Discrimination by English-Learning 5-Month-Olds: Effects of Rhythm and Familiarity , 2000 .
[193] S. Gelman,et al. How two-year-old children interpret proper and common names for unfamiliar objects. , 1984, Child development.
[194] Noam Chomsky,et al. Lectures on Government and Binding , 1981 .
[195] Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al. A moment of silence: How the prosodic cues in motherese might assist language learning , 1986 .
[196] Dare A. Baldwin,et al. Infants' contribution to the achievement of joint reference. , 1991, Child development.
[197] W. Bruce Croft. Typology and Universals , 1990 .
[198] Barbara Landau,et al. Objects and places: Geometric and syntactic representations in early lexical learning , 1990 .
[199] J R Saffran,et al. Emerging integration of sequential and suprasegmental information in preverbal speech segmentation. , 1995, Child development.
[200] S. Levinson,et al. Rethinking Linguistic Relativity , 1991, Current Anthropology.
[201] N. Soja. Inferences about the meanings of nouns: The relationship between perception and syntax , 1992 .
[202] Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al. Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults , 1999, Cognition.
[203] P. Jusczyk. The discovery of spoken language , 1997 .
[204] Lois Bloom,et al. Language acquisition in its developmental context , 1998 .
[205] H. Gleitman,et al. Linguistics: The Cambridge Survey: Where learning begins: initial representations for language learning , 1988 .
[206] Lila R. Gleitman,et al. What is a language made out of , 1997 .
[207] K. Bock,et al. From conceptual roles to structural relations: bridging the syntactic cleft. , 1992 .
[208] A. Samuel. Lexical Activation Produces Potent Phonemic Percepts , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.
[209] P. Bloom. How children learn the meanings of words , 2000 .
[210] John Macnamara,et al. What's in a name? A study of how children learn common and proper names. , 1974 .
[211] E. Markman,et al. Sixteen- and 24-month-olds' use of mutual exclusivity as a default assumption in second-label learning , 1994 .
[212] G. Miller,et al. The verb as the main determinant of sentence meaning , 1970 .
[213] P. Jusczyk,et al. A precursor of language acquisition in young infants , 1988, Cognition.
[214] I. M. Schlesinger,et al. Categories and Processes in Language Acquisition , 1990 .
[215] E. Spelke,et al. Origins of knowledge. , 1992, Psychological review.
[216] A. Goldberg. Constructions: A Construction Grammar Approach to Argument Structure , 1995 .
[217] Sandra R. Waxman,et al. Specifying the scope of 13-month-olds' expectations for novel words , 1999, Cognition.
[218] G. Dell,et al. Speech errors, phonotactic constraints, and implicit learning: a study of the role of experience in language production. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[219] Eric Wanner,et al. Language acquisition: the state of the art , 1982 .
[220] S. Carey. The child as word learner , 1978 .
[221] M. Tomasello,et al. Fourteen-through 18-month-old infants di eren-tially imitate intentional and accidental actions , 1998 .
[222] P. Quinn,et al. A Reexamination of the Perceptual-to-Conceptual Shift in Mental Representations , 1997 .
[223] H. Gleitman,et al. Mother, Id rather do it myself: Some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style , 1977 .
[224] S. Trehub. The Discrimination of Foreign Speech Contrasts by Infants and Adults. , 1976 .
[225] Anna Papafragou,et al. Shake, rattle, ‘n’ roll: the representation of motion in language and cognition , 2002, Cognition.
[226] L. Nakatani,et al. Locus of segmental cues for word juncture. , 1977, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[227] Ellen M. Markman,et al. Early Word Learning , 1997 .
[228] David R. Dowty. Thematic proto-roles and argument selection , 1991 .
[229] Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al. The Origins of Grammar: Evidence from Early Language Comprehension , 1999 .
[230] R. Brown. A First Language , 1973 .
[231] N. Sebastián-Gallés,et al. Native-language recognition abilities in 4-month-old infants from monolingual and bilingual environments , 1997, Cognition.
[232] P. Kuhl,et al. Enhanced discriminability at the phonetic boundaries for the place feature in macaques. , 1983, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[233] Kenneth Wexler,et al. Formal Principles of Language Acquisition , 1980 .
[234] Andrew Lotto,et al. Language Acquisition as Complex Category Formation , 2000, Phonetica.