Deriving the number of salience maps an observer has from the number and quality of concurrent centroid judgments.

[C. Koch, S. Ullman, Hum. Neurobiol.4, 219-227 (1985)] proposed a 2D topographical salience map that took feature-map outputs as its input and represented the importance "saliency" of the feature inputs at each location as a real number. The computation on the map, "winner-take-all," was used to predict action priority. We propose that the same or a similar map is used to compute centroid judgments, the center of a cloud of diverse items. [P. Sun, V. Chu, G. Sperling, Atten. Percept. Psychophys.83, 934-955 (2021)] demonstrated that following a 250-msec exposure of a 24-dot array of 3 intermixed colors, subjects could accurately report the centroid of each dot color, thereby indicating that these subjects had at least three salience maps. Here, we use a postcue, partial-report paradigm to determine how many more salience maps subjects might have. In 11 experiments, subjects viewed 0.3-s flashes of 28 to 32 item arrays composed of M, M = 3,...,8, different features followed by a cue to mouse-click the centroid of items of just the post-cued feature. Ideal detector response analyses show that subjects utilized at least 12 to 17 stimulus items. By determining whether a subject's performance in (M-1)-feature experiments could/could-not predict performance in M-feature experiments, we conclude that one subject has at least 7 and the other two have at least five salience maps. A computational model shows that the primary performance-limiting factors are channel capacity for representing so many concurrently presented groups of items and working-memory capacity for so many computed centroids.

[1]  G. Sperling,et al.  Two-dimensional shape perception is based on a salience map. , 2022, Journal of Vision.

[2]  George Sperling,et al.  Frontal-plane distance judgments between two equal-size items are made on the basis of a salience map , 2021, Journal of Vision.

[3]  Veronica C Chu,et al.  Multiple concurrent centroid judgments imply multiple within-group salience maps , 2021, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[4]  George Sperling,et al.  High-capacity preconscious processing in concurrent groupings of colored dots , 2018, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[5]  Jiaying Zhao,et al.  Capacity limit of ensemble perception of multiple spatially intermixed sets , 2018, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[6]  George Sperling,et al.  Human attention filters for single colors , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[7]  C. Moore,et al.  The capacity limitations of orientation summary statistics , 2015, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[8]  Yaoda Xu,et al.  The association of color memory and the enumeration of multiple spatially overlapping sets. , 2013, Journal of vision.

[9]  George Sperling,et al.  Quantifying attention: Attention filtering in centroid estimations , 2010 .

[10]  Peyman Milanfar,et al.  Static and space-time visual saliency detection by self-resemblance. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[11]  B. Bauer Does Stevens’s Power Law for Brightness Extend to Perceptual Brightness Averaging? , 2009 .

[12]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Automatic statistical processing of visual properties in simultanagnosia , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  Jason M Haberman,et al.  Correspondences Rapid extraction of mean emotion and gender from sets of faces , 2007 .

[14]  L. Feigenson,et al.  Multiple Spatially Overlapping Sets Can Be Enumerated in Parallel , 2006, Psychological science.

[15]  Pierre Baldi,et al.  Bayesian surprise attracts human attention , 2005, Vision Research.

[16]  John K. Tsotsos,et al.  Saliency Based on Information Maximization , 2005, NIPS.

[17]  A. Treisman,et al.  Representation of statistical properties , 2003, Vision Research.

[18]  Samiran Nundy,et al.  Thinking the unthinkable! , 2002, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[19]  D. Ariely Seeing Sets: Representation by Statistical Properties , 2001, Psychological science.

[20]  Christof Koch,et al.  A Model of Saliency-Based Visual Attention for Rapid Scene Analysis , 2009 .

[21]  Scott N. J. Watamaniuk,et al.  Visual search for motion-in-depth: stereomotion does not 'pop out' from disparity noise , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[22]  George Sperling,et al.  Attention-generated apparent motion , 1995, Nature.

[23]  B. Efron Computers and the Theory of Statistics: Thinking the Unthinkable , 1979 .

[24]  G. Sperling A Model for Visual Memory Tasks1 , 1963, Human factors.

[25]  D. Whitney,et al.  Ensemble Perception , 2018, Annual review of psychology.

[26]  S. Ullman,et al.  Shifts in selective visual attention: towards the underlying neural circuitry. , 1985, Human neurobiology.

[27]  George Sperling,et al.  The information available in brief visual presentations. , 1960 .