Magnitude knowledge: the common core of numerical development.

The integrated theory of numerical development posits that a central theme of numerical development from infancy to adulthood is progressive broadening of the types and ranges of numbers whose magnitudes are accurately represented. The process includes four overlapping trends: (1) representing increasingly precisely the magnitudes of non-symbolic numbers, (2) connecting small symbolic numbers to their non-symbolic referents, (3) extending understanding from smaller to larger whole numbers, and (4) accurately representing the magnitudes of rational numbers. The present review identifies substantial commonalities, as well as differences, in these four aspects of numerical development. With both whole and rational numbers, numerical magnitude knowledge is concurrently correlated with, longitudinally predictive of, and causally related to multiple aspects of mathematical understanding, including arithmetic and overall math achievement. Moreover, interventions focused on increasing numerical magnitude knowledge often generalize to other aspects of mathematics. The cognitive processes of association and analogy seem to play especially large roles in this development. Thus, acquisition of numerical magnitude knowledge can be seen as the common core of numerical development.

[1]  F. Galton I.—STATISTICS OF MENTAL IMAGERY , 1880 .

[2]  Arnold J. Toynbee A Study of History , 1955 .

[3]  F Mechner,et al.  Probability Relations within Response Sequences under Ratio Reinforcement. , 1958, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[4]  J. Piaget The Child's Conception of Number , 1953 .

[5]  S.M. Harris,et al.  Information Processing , 1977, Nature.

[6]  ROBERT S. MOYER,et al.  Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality , 1967, Nature.

[7]  A. Parducci The relativism of absolute judgements. , 1968, Scientific American.

[8]  Jean Piaget,et al.  The child's conception of time; , 1969 .

[9]  F. Restle Speed of Adding and Comparing Numbers. , 1970 .

[10]  John R. Platt,et al.  Localization of position within a homogeneous behavior chain: Effects of error contingencies , 1971 .

[11]  A. Newell You can't play 20 questions with nature and win : projective comments on the papers of this symposium , 1973 .

[12]  Benson Schaeffer,et al.  Number development in young children , 1974 .

[13]  Robert Sekuler,et al.  Children's Judgments of Numerical Inequality. , 1977 .

[14]  C. Gallistel,et al.  The Child's Understanding of Number , 1979 .

[15]  A. Henik,et al.  Is three greater than five: The relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks , 1982, Memory & cognition.

[16]  Robert S. Siegler,et al.  The Development of Numerical Understandings , 1982 .

[17]  Robert S. Siegler,et al.  A featural analysis of preschoolers' counting knowledge. , 1984 .

[18]  R. Newman,et al.  Children's Numerical Estimation: Flexibility in the Use of Counting. , 1984 .

[19]  U. Goswami,et al.  Relational complexity and the development of analogical reasoning , 1989 .

[20]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  Emmanuel Dupoux,et al.  Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. , 1990 .

[22]  K. Wynn Children's acquisition of the number words and the counting system , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[23]  D. J. Murray,et al.  A perspective for viewing the history of psychophysics , 1993, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[24]  C. Packer,et al.  Roaring and numerical assessment in contests between groups of female lions, Panthera leo , 1994, Animal Behaviour.

[25]  Karen Wynn,et al.  Infants Possess a System of Numerical Knowledge , 1995 .

[26]  U. Goswami,et al.  Analogical reasoning and cognitive development. , 1996, Advances in child development and behavior.

[27]  D. Gentner,et al.  Structure mapping in analogy and similarity. , 1997 .

[28]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics. , 1998 .

[29]  Peter Brugger,et al.  Stimulus-response compatibility in representational space , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  Steven A. Hecht,et al.  Toward an information processing account of individual differences in fraction skills , 1998 .

[31]  Haines What's past is prologue , 1998, The Canadian nurse.

[32]  Paul Guyer,et al.  Critique of Pure Reason: Transcendental doctrine of method , 1998 .

[33]  D. Berch,et al.  Extracting parity and magnitude from Arabic numerals: developmental changes in number processing and mental representation. , 1999, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[34]  J. Moss Developing Children's Understanding of the Rational Numbers: A New Model and an Experimental Curriculum , 1999 .

[35]  E. Spelke,et al.  Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants , 2000, Cognition.

[36]  Mary Gauvain,et al.  The social context of cognitive development , 2000 .

[37]  N. Fujimura,et al.  Facilitating children's proportional reasoning: A model of reasoning processes and effects of intervention on strategy change. , 2001 .

[38]  B. Rittle-Johnson,et al.  Developing Conceptual Understanding and Procedural Skill in Mathematics: An Iterative Process. , 2001 .

[39]  Yujing Ni,et al.  Semantic Domains of Rational Numbers and the Acquisition of Fraction Equivalence. , 2001, Contemporary educational psychology.

[40]  Klaus Willmes,et al.  Decade breaks in the mental number line? Putting the tens and units back in different bins , 2001, Cognition.

[41]  E. Spelke,et al.  Infants' Discrimination of Number vs. Continuous Extent , 2002, Cognitive Psychology.

[42]  Elizabeth S Spelke,et al.  Origins of Number Sense , 2003, Psychological science.

[43]  S. Dehaene The neural bases of subliminal priming , 2003 .

[44]  R. Siegler,et al.  The Development of Numerical Estimation , 2003, Psychological science.

[45]  Steven A Hecht,et al.  Sources of individual differences in fraction skills. , 2003, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[46]  Philippe Pinel,et al.  Tuning Curves for Approximate Numerosity in the Human Intraparietal Sulcus , 2004, Neuron.

[47]  Andreas Nieder,et al.  A parieto-frontal network for visual numerical information in the monkey. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[48]  S. Carey The Origin of Concepts , 2000 .

[49]  Wim Fias,et al.  Representation of Number in Animals and Humans: A Neural Model , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[50]  E. Spelke,et al.  Language and Conceptual Development series Core systems of number , 2004 .

[51]  S. Vosniadou,et al.  The development of students’ understanding of the numerical value of fractions , 2004 .

[52]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  Development of numerical estimation in young children. , 2004, Child development.

[53]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[54]  Kazuo Hiraki,et al.  Auditory-visual intermodal matching of small numerosities in 6-month-old infants. , 2005, Developmental science.

[55]  K. Willmes,et al.  The universal SNARC effect: the association between number magnitude and space is amodal. , 2005, Experimental psychology.

[56]  Anthony Lutkus,et al.  The Nation's Report Card. NAEP 2004 Trends in Academic Progress: Three Decades of Student Performance in Reading, 1971-2004 and Mathematics, 1973-2004. NCES 2005?464. , 2005 .

[57]  S. Dehaene,et al.  An open trial assessment of "The Number Race", an adaptive computer game for remediation of dyscalculia , 2006, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[58]  M. Pesenti,et al.  Numerosity-duration interference: a Stroop experiment. , 2006, Acta psychologica.

[59]  Karen Wynn,et al.  Six-month-old infants use analog magnitudes to represent duration. , 2006, Developmental science.

[60]  Elizabeth M Brannon,et al.  The multisensory representation of number in infancy. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[61]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  Developmental and individual differences in pure numerical estimation. , 2006, Developmental psychology.

[62]  Sara Cordes,et al.  The development of area discrimination and its implications for number representation in infancy. , 2006, Developmental science.

[63]  E. J. Carter,et al.  Functional Imaging of Numerical Processing in Adults and 4-y-Old Children , 2006, PLoS biology.

[64]  L. Lenchik Functional imaging , 2007, Annals of Biomedical Engineering.

[65]  Andreas Nieder,et al.  A Labeled-Line Code for Small and Large Numerosities in the Monkey Prefrontal Cortex , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[66]  Koleen McCrink,et al.  Ratio Abstraction by 6-Month-Old Infants , 2007, Psychological science.

[67]  Chung-Ming Chen,et al.  Exploring Group-Wise Conceptual Deficiencies of Fractions for Fifth and Sixth Graders in Taiwan , 2007 .

[68]  David C. Geary,et al.  Development of Mathematical Understanding. , 2007 .

[69]  Mary K. Hoard,et al.  Cognitive mechanisms underlying achievement deficits in children with mathematical learning disability. , 2007, Child development.

[70]  Susan Carey,et al.  One, two, three, four, nothing more: An investigation of the conceptual sources of the verbal counting principles , 2007, Cognition.

[71]  S. Dehaene,et al.  A Magnitude Code Common to Numerosities and Number Symbols in Human Intraparietal Cortex , 2007, Neuron.

[72]  S. Dehaene Symbols and quantities in parietal cortex: elements of a mathematical theory of number representation and manipulation , 2007 .

[73]  Elida V. Laski,et al.  Is 27 a big number? Correlational and causal connections among numerical categorization, number line estimation, and numerical magnitude comparison. , 2007, Child development.

[74]  Robert S. Siegler,et al.  Representational change and children’s numerical estimation , 2007, Cognitive Psychology.

[75]  T. Verguts,et al.  Dissecting the symbolic distance effect: Comparison and priming effects in numerical and nonnumerical orders , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[76]  Andreas Nieder,et al.  Behavioral and Prefrontal Representation of Spatial Proportions in the Monkey , 2008, Current Biology.

[77]  Rebecca Bull,et al.  Number games, magnitude representation, and basic number skills in preschoolers. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[78]  R. Siegler,et al.  Chinese Children Excel on Novel Mathematics Problems Even Before Elementary School , 2008, Psychological science.

[79]  Julie Sarama,et al.  Experimental Evaluation of the Effects of a Research-Based Preschool Mathematics Curriculum , 2008 .

[80]  Roi Cohen Kadosh,et al.  Are numbers special? An overview of chronometric, neuroimaging, developmental and comparative studies of magnitude representation , 2008, Progress in Neurobiology.

[81]  E. Brannon,et al.  The difficulties of representing continuous extent in infancy: using number is just easier. , 2008, Child development.

[82]  Guilherme Wood,et al.  On the Cognitive Link between Space and Number: A Meta-Analysis of the SNARC Effect , 2008 .

[83]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Individual differences in non-verbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement , 2008, Nature.

[84]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Does Subitizing Reflect Numerical Estimation? , 2022 .

[85]  Pieter Reitsma,et al.  Developing access to number magnitude: a study of the SNARC effect in 7- to 9-year-olds. , 2008, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[86]  Robbie Case,et al.  Comprar The Role of Central Conceptual Structures in the Development of Children's Thought | Robbie Case | 9780631224518 | Wiley , 2008 .

[87]  Geetha B. Ramani,et al.  Playing linear numerical board games promotes low-income children's numerical development. , 2008, Developmental science.

[88]  Marie-Pascale Noël,et al.  The development of automatic numerosity processing in preschoolers: evidence for numerosity-perceptual interference. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[89]  C. Gallistel,et al.  The generative basis of natural number concepts , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[90]  David C. Geary,et al.  Development of Number Line Representations in Children With Mathematical Learning Disability , 2008, Developmental neuropsychology.

[91]  David C. Geary,et al.  An Evolutionarily Informed Education Science , 2008 .

[92]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Domain-specific and domain-general changes in children's development of number comparison. , 2008, Developmental science.

[93]  Geetha B. Ramani,et al.  Promoting broad and stable improvements in low-income children's numerical knowledge through playing number board games. , 2008, Child development.

[94]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Developmental change in the acuity of the "Number Sense": The Approximate Number System in 3-, 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds and adults. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[95]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  Numerical magnitude representations influence arithmetic learning. , 2008, Child development.

[96]  Andreas Nieder,et al.  Notation-Independent Representation of Fractions in the Human Parietal Cortex , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[97]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Representation of number in the brain. , 2009, Annual review of neuroscience.

[98]  E. Spelke,et al.  Newborn infants perceive abstract numbers , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[99]  Marie-Pascale Noël,et al.  Rational numbers: Componential versus holistic representation of fractions in a magnitude comparison task , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[100]  M. Schocke,et al.  The processing and representation of fractions within the brain. An fMRI investigation. , 2009, NeuroImage.

[101]  G. Burghardt,et al.  Playing Linear Number Board Games — But Not Circular Ones — Improves Low-Income Preschoolers ’ Numerical Understanding , 2009 .

[102]  Bertrand Thirion,et al.  Deciphering Cortical Number Coding from Human Brain Activity Patterns , 2009, Current Biology.

[103]  Michael Schneider,et al.  Mental number line, number line estimation, and mathematical achievement : Their interrelations in grades 5 and 6 , 2009 .

[104]  Kristina Moll,et al.  Dyslexia and dyscalculia: two learning disorders with different cognitive profiles. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[105]  Mathematics Skills of 17-Year-Olds in the United States: 1978 to 2004. , 2010 .

[106]  Stella F. Lourenco,et al.  General Magnitude Representation in Human Infants , 2010, Psychological science.

[107]  Marie-Pascale Noël,et al.  Comparing the magnitude of two fractions with common components: which representations are used by 10- and 12-year-olds? , 2010, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[108]  Marco Zorzi,et al.  Numerical estimation in preschoolers. , 2010, Developmental psychology.

[109]  Manuela Piazza,et al.  Neurocognitive start-up tools for symbolic number representations , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[110]  P. Bryant,et al.  Individual differences in conceptual and procedural knowledge when learning fractions. , 2010 .

[111]  Mauro Pesenti,et al.  Place and summation coding for canonical and non-canonical finger numeral representations , 2010, Cognition.

[112]  Benjamin M. Rottman,et al.  Parental numeric language input to Mandarin Chinese and English speaking preschool children* , 2010, Journal of Child Language.

[113]  Michael Schneider,et al.  Representations of the magnitudes of fractions. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[114]  Melissa E. Libertus,et al.  Stable individual differences in number discrimination in infancy. , 2010, Developmental science.

[115]  James W. Stigler,et al.  What Community College Developmental Mathematics Students Understand about Mathematics. , 2010 .

[116]  Dana Ganor-Stern,et al.  Holistic Representation of Negative Numbers is Formed When Needed for the Task , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[117]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  Non-symbolic arithmetic abilities and mathematics achievement in the first year of formal schooling , 2010, Cognition.

[118]  Steven A Hecht,et al.  Sources of Group and Individual Differences in Emerging Fraction Skills. , 2010, Journal of educational psychology.

[119]  Clarissa A. Thompson,et al.  How 15 hundred is like 15 cherries: effect of progressive alignment on representational changes in numerical cognition. , 2010, Child development.

[120]  Martin H. Fischer,et al.  When Digits Help Digits: Spatial–Numerical Associations Point to Finger Counting as Prime Example of Embodied Cognition , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[121]  Manuela Piazza,et al.  Chapter 17 – Neurocognitive Start-Up Tools for Symbolic Number Representations* , 2011 .

[122]  Clarissa A. Thompson,et al.  An integrated theory of whole number and fractions development , 2011, Cognitive Psychology.

[123]  D. Geary,et al.  Consequences, Characteristics, and Causes of Mathematical Learning Disabilities and Persistent Low Achievement in Mathematics , 2011, Journal of developmental and behavioral pediatrics : JDBP.

[124]  Stella F. Lourenco,et al.  Origins and Development of Generalized Magnitude Representation , 2011 .

[125]  Melissa E. Libertus,et al.  Preschool acuity of the approximate number system correlates with school math ability. , 2011, Developmental science.

[126]  Hilary C Barth,et al.  The development of numerical estimation: evidence against a representational shift. , 2011, Developmental science.

[127]  Ulrike Cress,et al.  Sensori-motor spatial training of number magnitude representation , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[128]  Andreas Nieder,et al.  Chapter 8 – The Neural Code for Number , 2011 .

[129]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  Space, time and number in the brain : searching for the foundations of mathematical thought , 2011 .

[130]  Daniel L. Schwartz,et al.  The mental representation of integers: An abstract-to-concrete shift in the understanding of mathematical concepts , 2011, Cognition.

[131]  Mary K. Hoard,et al.  Competence with fractions predicts gains in mathematics achievement. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[132]  K. Holyoak,et al.  The Oxford handbook of thinking and reasoning , 2012 .

[133]  Emmy Defever,et al.  Association between basic numerical abilities and mathematics achievement. , 2012, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[134]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  The relation between spatial skill and early number knowledge: the role of the linear number line. , 2012, Developmental psychology.

[135]  M. Ashcraft,et al.  Cognitive processes of numerical estimation in children. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[136]  Michael Andres,et al.  Common substrate for mental arithmetic and finger representation in the parietal cortex , 2012, NeuroImage.

[137]  Andreas Nieder,et al.  Relating magnitudes: the brain's code for proportions , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[138]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  Fractions: Could they really be the gatekeeper’s doorman? , 2012 .

[139]  Margaret M. Gullick,et al.  Understanding less than nothing: Neural distance effects for negative numbers , 2012, NeuroImage.

[140]  K. Holyoak Analogy and Relational Reasoning , 2012 .

[141]  Peter Bryant,et al.  Individual differences in conceptual and procedural fraction understanding: the role of abilities and school experience. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[142]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Number sense across the lifespan as revealed by a massive Internet-based sample , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[143]  R. Siegler,et al.  Early Predictors of High School Mathematics Achievement , 2012, Psychological science.

[144]  Fruzsina Soltész,et al.  Symbolic Number: The Integration of Magnitude and Spatial Representations in Children Aged 6 to 8 Years , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[145]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Evidence for Two Numerical Systems That Are Similar in Humans and Guppies , 2012, PloS one.

[146]  Geetha B. Ramani,et al.  Taking it to the classroom: Number board games as a small group learning activity , 2012 .

[147]  Peter P. J. L. Verkoeijen,et al.  Multiple Representations in Number Line Estimation: A Developmental Shift or Classes of Representations? , 2012 .

[148]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  Malleability of the approximate number system: effects of feedback and training , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[149]  Gero Miesenböck,et al.  Odor Discrimination in Drosophila: From Neural Population Codes to Behavior , 2013, Neuron.

[150]  Jo-Anne LeFevre,et al.  Charting the role of the number line in mathematical development , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[151]  Stuart J. Ritchie,et al.  Enduring Links From Childhood Mathematics and Reading Achievement to Adult Socioeconomic Status , 2013, Psychology Science.

[152]  Aryn A Pyke,et al.  Developmental and individual differences in understanding of fractions. , 2013, Developmental psychology.

[153]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Links Between the Intuitive Sense of Number and Formal Mathematics Ability. , 2013, Child development perspectives.

[154]  H. Barth,et al.  Developmental change in numerical estimation. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[155]  Evelyn H. Kroesbergen,et al.  Working memory and mathematics in primary school children: A meta-analysis , 2013 .

[156]  Russell Gersten,et al.  Developmental predictors of fraction concepts and procedures. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[157]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  Visual capture and the experience of having two bodies – Evidence from two different virtual reality techniques , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[158]  Margaret M. Gullick,et al.  Understanding less than nothing: children's neural response to negative numbers shifts across age and accuracy , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[159]  J. Towse,et al.  Plane thinking: mental representations in number line estimation as a function of orientation, scale, and counting proficiency. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[160]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Open questions and a proposal: A critical review of the evidence on infant numerical abilities , 2013, Cognition.

[161]  Stella F. Lourenco,et al.  The approximate number system and its relation to early math achievement: evidence from the preschool years. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[162]  Number Line Estimation in Children with Developmental Dyscalculia. , 2013 .

[163]  Elizabeth M Brannon,et al.  Training the Approximate Number System Improves Math Proficiency , 2013, Psychological science.

[164]  Lynn S. Fuchs,et al.  Improving At-Risk Learners' Understanding of Fractions , 2013 .

[165]  Na Li,et al.  Development of numerical estimation in Chinese preschool children. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[166]  M. Just,et al.  Decoding the representation of numerical values from brain activation patterns , 2013, Human brain mapping.

[167]  C. Schiltz,et al.  Developing number-space associations: SNARC effects using a color discrimination task in 5-year-olds. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[168]  L. Piffer,et al.  Large Number Discrimination in Newborn Fish , 2013, PloS one.

[169]  Lynn S. Fuchs,et al.  Does working memory moderate the effects of fraction intervention? An aptitude–treatment interaction. , 2014 .

[170]  R. Siegler,et al.  What’s Past Is Prologue , 2014, Educational researcher.

[171]  David Barner,et al.  The development of structural analogy in number-line estimation. , 2014, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[172]  Qixuan Chen,et al.  Association between individual differences in non-symbolic number acuity and math performance: a meta-analysis. , 2014, Acta psychologica.

[173]  J. Bulthé,et al.  Format-dependent representations of symbolic and non-symbolic numbers in the human cortex as revealed by multi-voxel pattern analyses , 2014, NeuroImage.

[174]  Korbinian Moeller,et al.  On the Relation between the Mental Number Line and Arithmetic Competencies , 2014, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[175]  Rebecca Bull,et al.  Executive Functioning and Mathematics Achievement , 2014 .

[176]  Elida V. Laski,et al.  Learning from number board games: you learn what you encode. , 2014, Developmental psychology.

[177]  Lisa K Fazio,et al.  Relations of different types of numerical magnitude representations to each other and to mathematics achievement. , 2014, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[178]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  The impact of fraction magnitude knowledge on algebra performance and learning. , 2014, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[179]  Gavin R. Price,et al.  Numerical predictors of arithmetic success in grades 1-6. , 2014, Developmental science.

[180]  Daniel C. Hyde,et al.  Brief non-symbolic, approximate number practice enhances subsequent exact symbolic arithmetic in children , 2014, Cognition.

[181]  Richard Cowan,et al.  The Contributions of Domain-General and Numerical Factors to Third-Grade Arithmetic Skills and Mathematical Learning Disability , 2013, Journal of educational psychology.

[182]  L. Fuchs,et al.  Sources of individual differences in children's understanding of fractions. , 2014, Child development.

[183]  Robert S Siegler,et al.  Early predictors of middle school fraction knowledge. , 2014, Developmental science.

[184]  Miriam Bassok,et al.  Magnitude comparison with different types of rational numbers. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[185]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  Improving arithmetic performance with number sense training: An investigation of underlying mechanism , 2014, Cognition.

[186]  Robert S. Siegler,et al.  An Integrative Theory of Numerical Development. , 2014 .

[187]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Qualitatively different coding of symbolic and nonsymbolic numbers in the human brain , 2015, Human brain mapping.

[188]  Russell Gersten,et al.  Development of fraction concepts and procedures in U.S. and Chinese children. , 2015, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[189]  Lynn S. Fuchs,et al.  Supported Self-Explaining during Fraction Intervention. , 2015 .

[190]  Ian M. Lyons,et al.  Foundations of children's numerical and mathematical skills: the roles of symbolic and nonsymbolic representations of numerical magnitude. , 2015, Advances in child development and behavior.

[191]  Giorgio Vallortigara,et al.  Response to Comments on “Number-space mapping in the newborn chick resembles humans’ mental number line” , 2015, Science.

[192]  Robert S. Siegler,et al.  Bridging the Gap: Fraction Understanding Is Central to Mathematics Achievement in Students from Three Different Continents. , 2015 .

[193]  Stella F. Lourenco On the Relation between Numerical and Non-Numerical Magnitudes: Evidence for a General Magnitude System , 2015 .

[194]  James R. Booth,et al.  Perceiving fingers in single-digit arithmetic problems , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[195]  Kelly S. Mix,et al.  Thinking about quantity: the intertwined development of spatial and numerical cognition. , 2015, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[196]  Jacob M. Paul,et al.  Longitudinal changes in young children’s 0–100 to 0–1000 number-line error signatures , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[197]  Lynn S. Fuchs,et al.  Effects of Intervention to Improve At-Risk Fourth Graders’ Understanding, Calculations, and Word Problems with Fractions , 2016, The Elementary School Journal.

[198]  Lisa K. Fazio,et al.  Strategy use and strategy choice in fraction magnitude comparison. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[199]  Clarissa A. Thompson,et al.  Free versus anchored numerical estimation: A unified approach , 2016, Cognition.

[200]  Erin Ottmar,et al.  Categories of Large Numbers in Line Estimation , 2017, Cogn. Sci..