Functional connectivity during a social emotion task in adolescents and in adults

In this fMRI study we investigated functional connectivity between components of the mentalising system during a social emotion task, using psychophysiological interaction (PPI) analysis. Ten adults (22–32 years) and 18 adolescents (11–18 years) were scanned while thinking about scenarios in which a social or a basic emotion would be experienced. Unlike basic emotions (such as disgust and fear), social emotions (such as embarrassment and guilt) require the representation of another’s mental states. In both adults and adolescents, an anterior rostral region of medial prefrontal cortex (arMPFC) involved in mentalising showed greater connectivity with the posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS) bordering on the temporo‐parietal junction (TPJ) and with anterior temporal cortex (ATC) during social than during basic emotion. This result provides novel evidence that components of the mentalising system interact functionally during a social emotion task. Furthermore, functional connectivity differed between adolescence and adulthood. The adolescent group showed stronger connectivity between arMPFC and pSTS/TPJ during social relative to basic emotion than did the adult group, suggestive of developmental changes in functional integration within the mentalising system.

[1]  Geoffrey Bird,et al.  Development during Adolescence of the Neural Processing of Social Emotion , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[2]  Deborah A. Yurgelun-Todd,et al.  Sex-Related Developmental Differences in the Lateralized Activation of the Prefrontal Cortex and Amygdala during Perception of Facial Affect , 2004, Perceptual and motor skills.

[3]  Paul M. Thompson,et al.  Sexual dimorphism of brain developmental trajectories during childhood and adolescence , 2007, NeuroImage.

[4]  C. Frith,et al.  Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[5]  D. Margulies,et al.  Development of anterior cingulate functional connectivity from late childhood to early adulthood. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[6]  C. Frith,et al.  Distinct regions of medial rostral prefrontal cortex supporting social and nonsocial functions. , 2007, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[7]  Robert T. Knight,et al.  Superior Temporal SulcusIt's My Area: Or Is It? , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[8]  Deborah A Yurgelun-Todd,et al.  Sex Differences in Cerebral Tissue Volume and Cognitive Performance during Adolescence , 2002, Psychological reports.

[9]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Thalamic and temporal cortex input to medial prefrontal cortex in rhesus monkeys , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  C. Frith,et al.  Adolescent development of the neural circuitry for thinking about intentions , 2007, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[11]  S. Petersen,et al.  Development of distinct control networks through segregation and integration , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[12]  Vince D. Calhoun,et al.  Functional neural networks underlying response inhibition in adolescents and adults , 2007, Behavioural Brain Research.

[13]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[14]  Dingguo Gao,et al.  A regulation role of the prefrontal cortex in the fist-edge-palm task: Evidence from functional connectivity analysis , 2008, NeuroImage.

[15]  T. Klingberg,et al.  Combined analysis of DTI and fMRI data reveals a joint maturation of white and grey matter in a fronto-parietal network. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[16]  Josef Perner,et al.  Do visual perspective tasks need theory of mind? , 2006, NeuroImage.

[17]  P. Yakovlev,et al.  The myelogenetic cycles of regional maturation of the brain , 1967 .

[18]  C. Frith,et al.  Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[19]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[20]  Vincent J Schmithorst,et al.  Developmental differences in white matter architecture between boys and girls , 2008, Human brain mapping.

[21]  P. Huttenlocher Synaptic density in human frontal cortex - developmental changes and effects of aging. , 1979, Brain research.

[22]  S. Blakemore The social brain in adolescence , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[23]  J. Decety,et al.  What you believe versus what you think they believe: a neuroimaging study of conceptual perspective‐taking , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[24]  F. Benes,et al.  Myelination of a key relay zone in the hippocampal formation occurs in the human brain during childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. , 1994, Archives of general psychiatry.

[25]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[26]  P. Huttenlocher,et al.  Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex , 1997, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[27]  H. Barbas,et al.  Medial Prefrontal Cortices Are Unified by Common Connections With Superior Temporal Cortices and Distinguished by Input From Memory‐Related Areas in the Rhesus Monkey , 1999, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[28]  Mirella Dapretto,et al.  Developmental changes in the neural basis of interpreting communicative intent. , 2006, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[29]  M. Hallett,et al.  Modeling other minds , 1995, Neuroreport.

[30]  V. Calhoun,et al.  Brain network dynamics during error commission , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[31]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Psychophysiological and Modulatory Interactions in Neuroimaging , 1997, NeuroImage.

[32]  H. Loos,et al.  Synaptogenesis in human visual cortex — evidence for synapse elimination during normal development , 1982, Neuroscience Letters.

[33]  Ivanei E. Bramati,et al.  The Moral Affiliations of Disgust: A Functional MRI Study , 2005, Cognitive and behavioral neurology : official journal of the Society for Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology.

[34]  R. Saxe,et al.  Making sense of another mind: The role of the right temporo-parietal junction , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  C. Stichel,et al.  The mouse MPTP model: gene expression changes in dopaminergic neurons , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[36]  T. Paus,et al.  Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[37]  S. Petersen,et al.  The maturing architecture of the brain's default network , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[38]  Claude Nahmias,et al.  Sex differences in functional activation patterns revealed by increased emotion processing demands , 2004, Neuroreport.