A Model of Online Temporal-Spatial Integration for Immediacy and Overrule in Discourse Comprehension
暂无分享,去创建一个
Peter Ford Dominey | Hiroshi Ishiguro | Takahisa Uchida | Nicolas Lair | H. Ishiguro | Nicolas Lair | Takahisa Uchida
[1] Stefano Fusi,et al. Why neurons mix: high dimensionality for higher cognition , 2016, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[2] Barak A. Pearlmutter. Gradient calculations for dynamic recurrent neural networks: a survey , 1995, IEEE Trans. Neural Networks.
[3] Edward W. Wlotko,et al. Going the Extra Mile: Effects of Discourse Context on Two Late Positivities During Language Comprehension , 2020, Neurobiology of Language.
[4] Eneko Agirre,et al. SemEval-2017 Task 1: Semantic Textual Similarity Multilingual and Crosslingual Focused Evaluation , 2017, *SEMEVAL.
[5] Thorsten Joachims,et al. Text Categorization with Support Vector Machines: Learning with Many Relevant Features , 1998, ECML.
[6] Mante S. Nieuwland,et al. When Peanuts Fall in Love: N400 Evidence for the Power of Discourse , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[7] M A Just,et al. A theory of reading: from eye fixations to comprehension. , 1980, Psychological review.
[8] Alex Graves,et al. Neural Turing Machines , 2014, ArXiv.
[9] Peter Ford Dominey,et al. A Model of Corticostriatal Plasticity for Learning Oculomotor Associations and Sequences , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[10] Nelson Cowan,et al. Working Memory Capacity , 2005 .
[11] S. Hallam,et al. Author Correction: A compendium of geochemical information from the Saanich Inlet water column , 2019, Scientific Data.
[12] Andreas Christmann,et al. Support vector machines , 2008, Data Mining and Knowledge Discovery Handbook.
[13] M. Kutas,et al. Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. , 1980, Science.
[14] Tom Michael Mitchell,et al. Predicting Human Brain Activity Associated with the Meanings of Nouns , 2008, Science.
[15] P. Goldman-Rakic. Circuitry of Primate Prefrontal Cortex and Regulation of Behavior by Representational Memory , 2011 .
[16] Wei Xu,et al. End-to-end learning of semantic role labeling using recurrent neural networks , 2015, ACL.
[17] G. E. Alexander,et al. Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.
[18] Manuel G. Calvo,et al. Working Memory Capacity and Time Course of Predictive Inferences , 2000, Memory.
[19] T. Carrell,et al. Central Auditory System Plasticity Associated with Speech Discrimination Training , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[20] Ming-Wei Chang,et al. BERT: Pre-training of Deep Bidirectional Transformers for Language Understanding , 2019, NAACL.
[21] Tomas Mikolov,et al. Bag of Tricks for Efficient Text Classification , 2016, EACL.
[22] Kara D. Federmeier,et al. Thirty years and counting: finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). , 2011, Annual review of psychology.
[23] Peter Ford Dominey. Complex sensory-motor sequence learning based on recurrent state representation and reinforcement learning , 1995, Biological Cybernetics.
[24] Katherine Nelson,et al. Wittgenstein and contemporary theories of word learning , 2009 .
[25] Ashish Vaswani,et al. Decoding the neural representation of story meanings across languages , 2017, Human brain mapping.
[26] Herbert Jaeger,et al. Reservoir computing approaches to recurrent neural network training , 2009, Comput. Sci. Rev..
[27] Colin M. Brown,et al. Semantic Integration in Sentences and Discourse: Evidence from the N400 , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[28] Harald Haas,et al. Harnessing Nonlinearity: Predicting Chaotic Systems and Saving Energy in Wireless Communication , 2004, Science.
[29] Kara D. Federmeier,et al. A Rose by Any Other Name: Long-Term Memory Structure and Sentence Processing , 1999 .
[30] John F. Kolen,et al. Gradient Calculations for Dynamic Recurrent Neural Networks , 2001 .
[31] Peter Ford Dominey,et al. Beyond the word and image: characteristics of a common meaning system for language and vision revealed by functional and structural imaging , 2015, NeuroImage.
[32] Allyson Ettinger,et al. Modeling N400 amplitude using vector space models of word representation , 2016, CogSci.
[33] Peter Ford Dominey,et al. Reservoir Computing Properties of Neural Dynamics in Prefrontal Cortex , 2016, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[34] M. Blignaut,et al. Towards a Transferable and Cost-Effective Plant AFLP Protocol , 2013, PloS one.
[35] Daniel Jurafsky,et al. Word embeddings quantify 100 years of gender and ethnic stereotypes , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[36] Hiroyuki Shindo,et al. Wikipedia2Vec: An Efficient Toolkit for Learning and Visualizing the Embeddings of Words and Entities from Wikipedia , 2020, EMNLP.
[37] P. Hagoort,et al. The interaction of discourse context and world knowledge in online sentence comprehension. Evidence from the N400 , 2007, Brain Research.
[38] Colin M. Brown,et al. The N400 as a function of the level of processing. , 1995, Psychophysiology.
[39] J. Elman,et al. Generalized event knowledge activation during online sentence comprehension. , 2012, Journal of memory and language.
[40] Manuel G. Calvo,et al. The time course of predictive inferences depends on contextual constraints , 2000 .
[41] Peter Hagoort,et al. How the brain makes sense beyond the processing of single words – An MEG study , 2019, NeuroImage.
[42] Yun Zhu,et al. Support vector machines and Word2vec for text classification with semantic features , 2015, 2015 IEEE 14th International Conference on Cognitive Informatics & Cognitive Computing (ICCI*CC).
[43] Marcel Adam Just,et al. A Model of the Time Course and Content of Reading , 1982, Cogn. Sci..
[44] W. Kintsch. The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: a construction-integration model. , 1988, Psychological review.
[45] Henry Markram,et al. Real-Time Computing Without Stable States: A New Framework for Neural Computation Based on Perturbations , 2002, Neural Computation.
[46] T. Landauer,et al. A Solution to Plato's Problem: The Latent Semantic Analysis Theory of Acquisition, Induction, and Representation of Knowledge. , 1997 .
[47] Peter Ford Dominey,et al. Real-Time Parallel Processing of Grammatical Structure in the Fronto-Striatal System: A Recurrent Network Simulation Study Using Reservoir Computing , 2013, PloS one.
[48] Xiao-Jing Wang,et al. The importance of mixed selectivity in complex cognitive tasks , 2013, Nature.
[49] A. Treisman. Perceptual grouping and attention in visual search for features and for objects. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[50] M. W. Shields. An Introduction to Automata Theory , 1988 .
[51] James L. McClelland,et al. Modelling the N400 brain potential as change in a probabilistic representation of meaning , 2018, Nature Human Behaviour.
[52] Jeffrey D. Ullman,et al. Introduction to Automata Theory, Languages and Computation , 1979 .
[53] S. Kastner,et al. Attention in the real world: toward understanding its neural basis , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[54] Robert Oostenveld,et al. A 204-subject multimodal neuroimaging dataset to study language processing , 2019, Scientific Data.
[55] Peter Hagoort,et al. Beyond the sentence given , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[56] Jeffrey Dean,et al. Distributed Representations of Words and Phrases and their Compositionality , 2013, NIPS.
[57] Jack L. Gallant,et al. A Continuous Semantic Space Describes the Representation of Thousands of Object and Action Categories across the Human Brain , 2012, Neuron.
[58] Matthew W. Crocker,et al. A Neurocomputational Model of the N400 and the P600 in Language Processing , 2016, Cognitive science.
[59] Christoph Haller,et al. Going the extra mile. , 2018, The Journal of thoracic and cardiovascular surgery.
[60] W. Kintsch,et al. Time course of priming for associate and inference words in a discourse context , 1988, Memory & cognition.
[61] A. Chun,et al. On the brain , 2007, Nature Nanotechnology.
[62] Hartmut Fitz,et al. Getting real about Semantic Illusions: Rethinking the functional role of the P600 in language comprehension , 2012, Brain Research.
[63] J. H. Neely. Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. , 1991 .
[64] Hiroyuki Shindo,et al. Studio Ousia's Quiz Bowl Question Answering System , 2018, ArXiv.
[65] A. Friederici. Event-related brain potential studies in language , 2004, Current neurology and neuroscience reports.
[66] Allyson Ettinger. What BERT Is Not: Lessons from a New Suite of Psycholinguistic Diagnostics for Language Models , 2019, Transactions of the Association for Computational Linguistics.
[67] Thomas L. Griffiths,et al. Supplementary Information for Natural Speech Reveals the Semantic Maps That Tile Human Cerebral Cortex , 2022 .
[68] Dušanka Lazarević,et al. Data from the Human Penguin Project, a cross-national dataset testing social thermoregulation principles , 2019, Scientific Data.
[69] Yoshua Bengio,et al. Neural Probabilistic Language Models , 2006 .
[70] T. Joachims,et al. Text Categorization with Support Vector Machines: Learning with Many Relevant Features , 1999, ECML.
[71] Peter Hagoort,et al. When Elephants Fly: Differential Sensitivity of Right and Left Inferior Frontal Gyri to Discourse and World Knowledge , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[72] Quoc V. Le,et al. Distributed Representations of Sentences and Documents , 2014, ICML.