Perceiving active listening activates the reward system and improves the impression of relevant experiences

Although active listening is an influential behavior, which can affect the social responses of others, the neural correlates underlying its perception have remained unclear. Sensing active listening in social interactions is accompanied by an improvement in the recollected impressions of relevant experiences and is thought to arouse positive feelings. We therefore hypothesized that the recognition of active listening activates the reward system, and that the emotional appraisal of experiences that had been subject to active listening would be improved. To test these hypotheses, we conducted functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) on participants viewing assessments of their own personal experiences made by evaluators with or without active listening attitude. Subjects rated evaluators who showed active listening more positively. Furthermore, they rated episodes more positively when they were evaluated by individuals showing active listening. Neural activation in the ventral striatum was enhanced by perceiving active listening, suggesting that this was processed as rewarding. It also activated the right anterior insula, representing positive emotional reappraisal processes. Furthermore, the mentalizing network was activated when participants were being evaluated, irrespective of active listening behavior. Therefore, perceiving active listening appeared to result in positive emotional appraisal and to invoke mental state attribution to the active listener.

[1]  G. Glover,et al.  Behavioral and Neural Properties of Social Reinforcement Learning , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  R. Zatorre,et al.  Anatomically distinct dopamine release during anticipation and experience of peak emotion to music , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  Eva H. Telzer,et al.  Gaining while giving: An fMRI study of the rewards of family assistance among White and Latino youth , 2010, Social neuroscience.

[4]  Adam Johnson,et al.  Triple Dissociation of Information Processing in Dorsal Striatum, Ventral Striatum, and Hippocampus on a Learned Spatial Decision Task , 2010, Neuron.

[5]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Neural correlates of anchoring-and-adjustment during mentalizing , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[6]  R. Vonk,et al.  Mimicry in social interaction: benefits for mimickers, mimickees, and their interaction. , 2010, British journal of psychology.

[7]  T. Singer,et al.  The role of anterior insular cortex in social emotions , 2010, Brain Structure and Function.

[8]  L. Shah,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 2010, Seminars in roentgenology.

[9]  Barbara C. N. Müller,et al.  Why do I like you when you behave like me? Neural mechanisms mediating positive consequences of observing someone being imitated , 2010, Social neuroscience.

[10]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Topological FDR for neuroimaging , 2010, NeuroImage.

[11]  C. Frith,et al.  Rapid Response Subject Collections , 2022 .

[12]  H. Critchley,et al.  A common role of insula in feelings, empathy and uncertainty , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[13]  R. Baaren,et al.  Effects of mimicking : Acting prosocially by being emotionally moved , 2008 .

[14]  N. Sadato,et al.  Processing of Social and Monetary Rewards in the Human Striatum , 2008, Neuron.

[15]  S. Quartz,et al.  Human Insula Activation Reflects Risk Prediction Errors As Well As Risk , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  M. Brass,et al.  Investigating Action Understanding: Inferential Processes versus Action Simulation , 2007, Current Biology.

[17]  K. Fliessbach,et al.  Social Comparison Affects Reward-Related Brain Activity in the Human Ventral Striatum , 2007, Science.

[18]  Hugo D. Critchley,et al.  Modulation of Emotional Appraisal by False Physiological Feedback during fMRI , 2007, PloS one.

[19]  M. Paulus,et al.  An Insular View of Anxiety , 2006, Biological Psychiatry.

[20]  C. Frith,et al.  Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[21]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Differential role of the orbital frontal lobe in emotional versus cognitive perspective-taking , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Reward representations and reward-related learning in the human brain: insights from neuroimaging , 2004, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[23]  Peter Dayan,et al.  Temporal difference models describe higher-order learning in humans , 2004, Nature.

[24]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dissociable Roles of Ventral and Dorsal Striatum in Instrumental Conditioning , 2004, Science.

[25]  Yul-Wan Sung,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2004, Scholarpedia.

[26]  K. Yau,et al.  Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body , 2003, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[27]  C. Frith,et al.  Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[28]  R. Elliott,et al.  Differential Response Patterns in the Striatum and Orbitofrontal Cortex to Financial Reward in Humans: A Parametric Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  A. Craig How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[30]  Uta Frith,et al.  Theory of mind , 2001, Current Biology.

[31]  Brian Knutson,et al.  Anticipation of Increasing Monetary Reward Selectively Recruits Nucleus Accumbens , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  A. Damasio,et al.  Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[33]  C. Frith,et al.  Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  C. Frith,et al.  Interacting minds--a biological basis. , 1999, Science.

[35]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Dissociating pain from its anticipation in the human brain. , 1999, Science.

[36]  T. Chartrand,et al.  The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[37]  Clara E. Hill,et al.  The current state of empathy research , 1996 .

[38]  R. Baumeister,et al.  The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. , 1995, Psychological bulletin.

[39]  A R Damasio,et al.  Descartes' error and the future of human life. , 1994, Scientific American.

[40]  C. Rogers The necessary and sufficient conditions of therapeutic personality change. , 1992, Psychotherapy.

[41]  G. Gladstein,et al.  Empathy and Counseling Outcome: An Empirical and Conceptual Review , 1977 .

[42]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  J. Singer,et al.  Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. , 1962, Psychological review.

[44]  W. James Discussion: The physical basis of emotion. , 1894 .

[45]  N. Sadato,et al.  Validity and Reliability of the Index of Active Listening (IAL) , 2013 .

[46]  C. Rogers A Theory of Therapy , Personality , and Interpersonal Relationships , as Developed in the Client-centered Framework , 2010 .

[47]  A. Craig,et al.  How do you feel — now? The anterior insula and human awareness , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[48]  R. Hoskins,et al.  Neural Responses to Taxation and Voluntary Giving Reveal Motives for Charitable Donations , 2007 .

[49]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .