Fact vs fiction--how paratextual information shapes our reading processes.

Our life is full of stories: some of them depict real-life events and were reported, e.g. in the daily news or in autobiographies, whereas other stories, as often presented to us in movies and novels, are fictional. However, we have only little insights in the neurocognitive processes underlying the reading of factual as compared to fictional contents. We investigated the neurocognitive effects of reading short narratives, labeled to be either factual or fictional. Reading in a factual mode engaged an activation pattern suggesting an action-based reconstruction of the events depicted in a story. This process seems to be past-oriented and leads to shorter reaction times at the behavioral level. In contrast, the brain activation patterns corresponding to reading fiction seem to reflect a constructive simulation of what might have happened. This is in line with studies on imagination of possible past or future events.

[1]  Demis Hassabis,et al.  Differential engagement of brain regions within a ‘core’ network during scene construction , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  A. Reboul,et al.  The logical status of fictional discourse: what Searle’s speaker can’t say to his hearer , 1990 .

[3]  C. Frith,et al.  Experiencing Oneself vs Another Person as Being the Cause of an Action: The Neural Correlates of the Experience of Agency , 2002, NeuroImage.

[4]  Andrew D. Engell,et al.  The Neural Bases of Cognitive Conflict and Control in Moral Judgment , 2004, Neuron.

[5]  Stephanie Spengler,et al.  Differential functions of lateral and medial rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) revealed by brain-behavior associations. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[6]  J. Decety,et al.  The functional architecture of human empathy. , 2004, Behavioral and cognitive neuroscience reviews.

[7]  J. Woolley,et al.  Development of beliefs about storybook reality. , 2007, Developmental science.

[8]  K. Hamburger The Logic of Literature , 1973 .

[9]  Jordan B. Peterson,et al.  Bookworms versus nerds: Exposure to fiction versus non-fiction, divergent associations with social ability, and the simulation of fictional social worlds , 2006 .

[10]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others , 2006, Nature.

[11]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Action outcomes are represented in human inferior frontoparietal cortex. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[12]  C. Frith,et al.  Neural correlates of observing pretend play in which one object is represented as another. , 2009, Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience.

[13]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dissociable Neural Responses in Human Reward Systems , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  D. Schacter,et al.  Constructive episodic simulation of the future and the past: Distinct subsystems of a core brain network mediate imagining and remembering , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Solving future problems: Default network and executive activity associated with goal-directed mental simulations , 2011, NeuroImage.

[16]  R. Gerrig,et al.  Psychological Processes Underlying Literary Impact , 2004 .

[17]  A. Jacobs,et al.  Gehirn und Gedicht : wie wir unsere Wirklichkeiten konstruieren , 2011 .

[18]  Keith Oatley,et al.  The Function of Fiction is the Abstraction and Simulation of Social Experience , 2008, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[19]  Kevin S. Brown,et al.  Cooperation between the default mode network and the frontal–parietal network in the production of an internal train of thought , 2012, Brain Research.

[20]  Katherine Maynard,et al.  Six walks in the fictional woods , 1995 .

[21]  D. Levin Thinking and seeing : visual metacognition in adults and children , 2004 .

[22]  Jiang Xu,et al.  Language in context: emergent features of word, sentence, and narrative comprehension , 2005, NeuroImage.

[23]  R. Mar The neuropsychology of narrative: story comprehension, story production and their interrelation , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  A PET Exploration of the Neural Mechanisms Involved in Reciprocal Imitation , 2002, NeuroImage.

[25]  D. Yves von Cramon,et al.  Thinking about the future versus the past in personal and non-personal contexts , 2008, Brain Research.

[26]  Demis Hassabis,et al.  Cortical midline involvement in autobiographical memory , 2009, NeuroImage.

[27]  Melanie C. Green,et al.  Transportation Into Narrative Worlds: The Role of Prior Knowledge and Perceived Realism , 2004 .

[28]  R. Veit,et al.  Regulation of emotional responses elicited by threat‐related stimuli , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[29]  Murray Smith Double Trouble: On Film, Fiction, and Narrative , 2010 .

[30]  Erik D. Reichle,et al.  Zoning Out while Reading: Evidence for Dissociations between Experience and Metaconsciousness. , 2004 .

[31]  Jan Voogd,et al.  The human central nervous system : a synopsis and atlas , 1978 .

[32]  Keith Oatley,et al.  On Being Moved by Art: How Reading Fiction Transforms the Self , 2009 .

[33]  C. Frith,et al.  Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[34]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Distinct neural substrates for the perception of real and virtual visual worlds , 2005, NeuroImage.

[35]  Steven L. Small,et al.  Interpretation-mediated changes in neural activity during language comprehension , 2011, NeuroImage.

[36]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Neural substrates of narrative comprehension and memory , 2008, NeuroImage.

[37]  Axel Schäfer,et al.  Emotion regulation in spider phobia: role of the medial prefrontal cortex. , 2009, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[38]  R. Mar The neural bases of social cognition and story comprehension. , 2011, Annual review of psychology.

[39]  K. Kiehl,et al.  Double dissociation between perspective-taking and empathic-concern as predictors of hemodynamic response to another's mistakes. , 2009, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[40]  N. A. Borghese,et al.  Different Brain Correlates for Watching Real and Virtual Hand Actions , 2001, NeuroImage.

[41]  C. Frith,et al.  The Neural Basis of Mentalizing , 2006, Neuron.

[42]  Peter Lamarque,et al.  Truth, Fiction, and Literature , 1996 .

[43]  Sharron E. Whitecross,et al.  Neurophysiological correlates of memory for experienced and imagined events , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[44]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Neural correlates of action attribution in schizophrenia , 2004, Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging.

[45]  G. Genette,et al.  Fictional Narrative, Factual Narrative , 1990 .

[46]  Mark H. Davis Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. , 1983 .

[47]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Gender Differences in Emotion Regulation: An fMRI Study of Cognitive Reappraisal , 2008, Group processes & intergroup relations : GPIR.

[48]  D. Yves von Cramon,et al.  Meeting George Bush versus Meeting Cinderella: The Neural Response When Telling Apart What is Real from What is Fictional in the Context of Our Reality , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[49]  J. Decety,et al.  Motor cognition: a new paradigm to study self–other interactions , 2004, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[50]  G. Pagnoni,et al.  Human Striatal Response to Salient Nonrewarding Stimuli , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network, supports goal-directed cognition , 2010, NeuroImage.

[52]  Nava Rubin,et al.  Brain areas selective for both observed and executed movements. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[53]  D. Shohamy,et al.  Ventromedial prefrontal-subcortical systems and the generation of affective meaning , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[54]  J. Decety,et al.  The power of simulation: Imagining one's own and other's behavior , 2006, Brain Research.

[55]  T. Heatherton,et al.  Detecting agency from the biological motion of veridical vs animated agents. , 2007, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[56]  Peter Lamarque,et al.  Truth, Fiction, and Literature: A Philosophical Perspective. , 1997 .

[57]  Uri Hasson,et al.  Brain networks subserving the extraction of sentence information and its encoding to memory. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[58]  H. Wellman,et al.  Young children's understanding of realities, nonrealities, and appearances. , 1990, Child development.

[59]  C. Frith,et al.  Recruitment of lateral rostral prefrontal cortex in spontaneous and task-related thoughts , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[60]  R. Passingham,et al.  Seeing or Doing? Influence of Visual and Motor Familiarity in Action Observation , 2006, Current Biology.

[61]  M. Symms,et al.  Imaging memory in temporal lobe epilepsy: predicting the effects of temporal lobe resection , 2010, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[62]  Roel M. Willems,et al.  Add a picture for suspense: neural correlates of the interaction between language and visual information in the perception of fear. , 2011, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[63]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Neural evidence for inequality-averse social preferences , 2010, Nature.

[64]  W. Kintsch,et al.  Strategies of discourse comprehension , 1983 .

[65]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Psychophysiological and Modulatory Interactions in Neuroimaging , 1997, NeuroImage.

[66]  Beate Sodian,et al.  Modulation of the cortical false belief network during development , 2010, Brain Research.

[67]  Lutz Jäncke,et al.  Feeling Present in Arousing Virtual Reality Worlds: Prefrontal Brain Regions Differentially Orchestrate Presence Experience in Adults and Children , 2008, Frontiers in human neuroscience.

[68]  Evelyn C. Ferstl,et al.  The extended language network: A meta‐analysis of neuroimaging studies on text comprehension , 2008, Human brain mapping.

[69]  G. Fernández,et al.  Reinforcement Learning Signal Predicts Social Conformity , 2009, Neuron.

[70]  Michael B. Miller,et al.  Neural Correlates of Detecting Pretense: Automatic Engagement of the Intentional Stance under Covert Conditions , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[71]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Analysis of functional image analysis contest (FIAC) data with brainvoyager QX: From single‐subject to cortically aligned group general linear model analysis and self‐organizing group independent component analysis , 2006, Human brain mapping.

[72]  Leif D. Nelson,et al.  The Virtues of Opaque Prose: How Lay Beliefs About Fluency Influence Perceptions of Quality , 2011 .

[73]  K. Oatley,et al.  Cues to the Imagination in Memoir, Science, and Fiction , 2010 .

[74]  Rolf A. Zwaan Effect of genre expectations on text comprehension. , 1994 .

[75]  Ana Raposo,et al.  Contributions of frontopolar cortex to judgments about self, others and relations. , 2011, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[76]  Evelyn C. Ferstl,et al.  What Does the Frontomedian Cortex Contribute to Language Processing: Coherence or Theory of Mind? , 2002, NeuroImage.

[77]  J. Smallwood,et al.  When attention matters: The curious incident of the wandering mind , 2008, Memory & cognition.

[78]  Erik D. Reichle,et al.  Eye Movements During Mindless Reading , 2010, Psychological science.

[79]  D. Schacter,et al.  Episodic Simulation of Future Events , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[80]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[81]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .