Media multitasking in adolescence

Media use has been on the rise in adolescents overall, and in particular, the amount of media multitasking—multiple media consumed simultaneously, such as having a text message conversation while watching TV—has been increasing. In adults, heavy media multitasking has been linked with poorer performance on a number of laboratory measures of cognition, but no relationship has yet been established between media-multitasking behavior and real-world outcomes. Examining individual differences across a group of adolescents, we found that more frequent media multitasking in daily life was associated with poorer performance on statewide standardized achievement tests of math and English in the classroom, poorer performance on behavioral measures of executive function (working memory capacity) in the laboratory, and traits of greater impulsivity and lesser growth mindset. Greater media multitasking had a relatively circumscribed set of associations, and was not related to behavioral measures of cognitive processing speed, implicit learning, or manual dexterity, or to traits of grit and conscientiousness. Thus, individual differences in adolescent media multitasking were related to specific differences in executive function and in performance on real-world academic achievement measures: More media multitasking was associated with poorer executive function ability, worse academic achievement, and a reduced growth mindset.

[1]  Kathryn M. McMillan,et al.  N‐back working memory paradigm: A meta‐analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[2]  Joshua Aronson,et al.  Improving adolescents' standardized test performance: An intervention to reduce the effects of stereotype threat , 2003 .

[3]  M. Farah,et al.  Childhood poverty: Specific associations with neurocognitive development , 2006, Brain Research.

[4]  C. Dweck Mindset: The New Psychology of Success , 2006 .

[5]  D. Wechsler Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children , 2020, Definitions.

[6]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Video game practice optimizes executive control skills in dual-task and task switching situations. , 2012, Acta psychologica.

[7]  S. Pickering,et al.  Working memory deficits in children with low achievements in the national curriculum at 7 years of age. , 2000, The British journal of educational psychology.

[8]  Susanne E. Baumgartner,et al.  The Relationship Between Media Multitasking and Executive Function in Early Adolescents , 2014 .

[9]  V. Rideout,et al.  Generation M2: Media in the Lives of 8- to 18-Year-Olds , 2010 .

[10]  S. Srivastava,et al.  The Big Five Trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. , 1999 .

[11]  M. Gluck,et al.  Probabilistic classification learning in amnesia. , 1994, Learning & memory.

[12]  Anastasia Kononova,et al.  Multitasking Across Borders: A Cross-National Study of Media Multitasking Behaviors, Its Antecedents, and Outcomes , 2013 .

[13]  Daphne Bavelier,et al.  Stretching the limits of visual attention: the case of action video games. , 2011, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[14]  Angela L. Duckworth,et al.  Domain-specific impulsivity in school-age children. , 2013, Developmental science.

[15]  Daniel A. Hackman,et al.  Socioeconomic status and executive function: developmental trajectories and mediation. , 2015, Developmental science.

[16]  Angela L. Duckworth,et al.  Development and Validation of the Short Grit Scale (Grit–S) , 2009, Journal of personality assessment.

[17]  O. John,et al.  Development of big five domains and facets in adulthood: mean-level age trends and broadly versus narrowly acting mechanisms. , 2012, Journal of personality.

[18]  M. Farah,et al.  Executive function as a mediator between SES and academic achievement throughout childhood , 2017, International journal of behavioral development.

[19]  Brittany R. L. Duff,et al.  Doing It All: An Exploratory Study of Predictors of Media Multitasking , 2014 .

[20]  Stephen Wee Hun Lim,et al.  Media multitasking predicts unitary versus splitting visual focal attention , 2013 .

[21]  R. Case,et al.  Operational efficiency and the growth of short-term memory span , 1982 .

[22]  Mark W. Becker,et al.  The association between media multitasking, task-switching, and dual-task performance. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[23]  Daphna Shohamy,et al.  Distinct Hippocampal and Basal Ganglia Contributions to Probabilistic Learning and Reversal , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[24]  Jonathan W. Peirce,et al.  PsychoPy—Psychophysics software in Python , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[25]  S. Gathercole,et al.  The cognitive and behavioral characteristics of children with low working memory. , 2009, Child development.

[26]  Matthew S Cain,et al.  Distractor filtering in media multitaskers. , 2010, Perception.

[27]  Tip Robertson,et al.  Reading While Watching Video: The Effect of Video Content on Reading Comprehension and Media Multitasking Ability , 2011 .

[28]  Clifford Nass,et al.  Cognitive control in media multitaskers , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[29]  M. Gluck,et al.  How do people solve the "weather prediction" task?: individual variability in strategies for probabilistic category learning. , 2002, Learning & memory.

[30]  Julia A. Leonard,et al.  Neuroanatomical Correlates of the Income-Achievement Gap , 2015, Psychological science.

[31]  Andrew R. A. Conway,et al.  Working memory, attention control, and the N-back task: a question of construct validity. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[32]  Brandon C. W. Ralph,et al.  Media multitasking and failures of attention in everyday life , 2014, Psychological research.

[33]  U. Foehr MEDIA MULTITASKING AMONG AMERICAN YOUTH: PREVALENCE, PREDICTORS AND PAIRINGS , 2006 .

[34]  Randall W Engle,et al.  Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent-variable approach. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[35]  Rebecca E. Martin,et al.  Promise and Paradox : Measuring Students ’ Non-Cognitive Skills and the Impact of Schooling , 2015 .

[36]  Mark W. Becker,et al.  Media Multitasking Is Associated with Symptoms of Depression and Social Anxiety , 2013, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..

[37]  Stephen R. Mitroff,et al.  Distractor Filtering in Media Multitaskers , 2011 .

[38]  James Gips,et al.  Media Multitasking Behavior: Concurrent Television and Computer Usage , 2011, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..

[39]  Julia A. Leonard,et al.  Cognitive Skills, Student Achievement Tests, and Schools , 2014, Psychological science.

[40]  D. Strayer,et al.  Who Multi-Tasks and Why? Multi-Tasking Ability, Perceived Multi-Tasking Ability, Impulsivity, and Sensation Seeking , 2013, PloS one.

[41]  X. Tolsa The capacity γ , 2014 .

[42]  Lisa S. Blackwell,et al.  Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: a longitudinal study and an intervention. , 2007, Child development.

[43]  Andrew R. A. Conway,et al.  On the capacity of attention: Its estimation and its role in working memory and cognitive aptitudes , 2005, Cognitive Psychology.

[44]  Lao Juan,et al.  Development and Validation of a Scale for Measuring Instructors' Attitudes toward Concept-Based or Reform-Oriented Teaching of Introductory Statistics in the Health and Behavioral Sciences , 2010, 1007.3219.

[45]  Meredith Minear,et al.  Working memory, fluid intelligence, and impulsiveness in heavy media multitaskers , 2013, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[46]  Matthew S. Cain,et al.  Improved control of exogenous attention in action video game players , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[47]  Angela L Duckworth,et al.  What No Child Left Behind Leaves Behind: The Roles of IQ and Self-Control in Predicting Standardized Achievement Test Scores and Report Card Grades. , 2012, Journal of educational psychology.