Performance Monitoring by the Anterior Cingulate Cortex During Saccade Countermanding

Consensus is emerging that the medial frontal lobe of the brain is involved in monitoring performance, but precisely what is monitored remains unclear. A saccade-countermanding task affords an experimental dissociation of neural signals of error, reinforcement, and conflict. Single-unit activity was monitored in the anterior cingulate cortex of monkeys performing this task. Neurons that signaled errors were found, half of which responded to the omission of earned reinforcement. A further diversity of neurons signaled earned or unexpected reinforcement. No neurons signaled the form of conflict engendered by interruption of saccade preparation produced in this task. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that the anterior cingulate cortex monitors the consequences of actions.

[1]  Masataka Watanabe,et al.  Prefrontal and cingulate unit activity during timing behavior in the monkey , 1979, Brain Research.

[2]  W. Becker,et al.  An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli , 1979, Vision Research.

[3]  G. Logan On the ability to inhibit thought and action , 1984 .

[4]  J. Kaas,et al.  Supplementary eye field as defined by intracortical microstimulation: Connections in macaques , 1990, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[5]  J D Schall,et al.  Topography of supplementary eye field afferents to frontal eye field in macaque: Implications for mapping between saccade coordinate systems , 1993, Visual Neuroscience.

[6]  J. Schall,et al.  Countermanding saccades in macaque , 1995, Visual Neuroscience.

[7]  J. Schall,et al.  Role of frontal eye fields in countermanding saccades: visual, movement, and fixation activity. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  J. Tanji,et al.  Role for cingulate motor area cells in voluntary movement selection based on reward. , 1998, Science.

[9]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  How the Basal Ganglia Use Parallel Excitatory and Inhibitory Learning Pathways to Selectively Respond to Unexpected Rewarding Cues , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  J. Schall,et al.  Performance monitoring by the supplementary eye ® eld , 2000 .

[11]  M. Posner,et al.  Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[13]  D. Wilkin,et al.  Neuron , 2001, Brain Research.

[14]  T. Paus Primate anterior cingulate cortex: Where motor control, drive and cognition interface , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[15]  Adrian R. Willoughby,et al.  The Medial Frontal Cortex and the Rapid Processing of Monetary Gains and Losses , 2002, Science.

[16]  W. Schultz Getting Formal with Dopamine and Reward , 2002, Neuron.

[17]  P. Dayan,et al.  Reward, Motivation, and Reinforcement Learning , 2002, Neuron.

[18]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.

[19]  B. Richmond,et al.  Anterior Cingulate: Single Neuronal Signals Related to Degree of Reward Expectancy , 2002, Science.

[20]  P. Montague,et al.  Neural Economics and the Biological Substrates of Valuation , 2002, Neuron.

[21]  D. P. Hanes,et al.  Controlled Movement Processing: Superior Colliculus Activity Associated with Countermanded Saccades , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  G. Luppino,et al.  ß Federation of European Neuroscience Societies Prefrontal and agranular cingulate projections to the dorsal premotor areas F2 and F7 in the macaque monkey , 2022 .

[23]  Robert M McPeek,et al.  Competition between saccade goals in the superior colliculus produces saccade curvature. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.