Memory shapes visual search strategies in large-scale environments

Search is a central visual function. Most of what is known about search derives from experiments where subjects view 2D displays on computer monitors. In the natural world, however, search involves movement of the body in large-scale spatial contexts, and it is unclear how this might affect search strategies. In this experiment, we explore the nature of memory representations developed when searching in an immersive virtual environment. By manipulating target location, we demonstrate that search depends on episodic spatial memory as well as learnt spatial priors. Subjects rapidly learned the large-scale structure of the space, with shorter paths and less head rotation to find targets. These results suggest that spatial memory of the global structure allows a search strategy that involves efficient attention allocation based on the relevance of scene regions. Thus spatial memory may allow less energetically costly search strategies.

[1]  Tom Foulsham,et al.  Top-down and bottom-up aspects of active search in a real-world environment. , 2014, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[2]  J. Wolfe,et al.  Five factors that guide attention in visual search , 2017, Nature Human Behaviour.

[3]  Matthew J. Stainer,et al.  Coordinating Vision and Action in Natural Behaviour: Differences in Spatiotemporal Coupling in Everyday Tasks , 2017, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[4]  Miguel P Eckstein,et al.  Object co-occurrence serves as a contextual cue to guide and facilitate visual search in a natural viewing environment. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[5]  James R. Brockmole,et al.  Contextual cueing in naturalistic scenes: Global and local contexts. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[6]  Hanspeter A Mallot,et al.  Head and eye movements and the role of memory limitations in a visual search paradigm. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[7]  J. Jonides,et al.  Evidence of hierarchies in cognitive maps , 1985, Memory & cognition.

[8]  J. Gottlieb Attention, Learning, and the Value of Information , 2012, Neuron.

[9]  N. Burgess Spatial Cognition and the Brain , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[10]  Russell A. Epstein,et al.  Schematic representations of local environmental space guide goal-directed navigation , 2017, Cognition.

[11]  Sun Ha Jee,et al.  Population-Attributable Causes of Cancer in Korea: Obesity and Physical Inactivity , 2014, PloS one.

[12]  Michael S Landy,et al.  Motor control is decision-making , 2012, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[13]  D. Newport,et al.  Transient natural convection in a conducting enclosure heated from above , 2013, J. Vis..

[14]  Miguel P. Eckstein,et al.  Gaze control and memory for objects while walking in a real world environment , 2009 .

[15]  Michael F. Land,et al.  The coordination of rotations of the eyes, head and trunk in saccadic turns produced in natural situations , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  Dejan Draschkow,et al.  Of “what” and “where” in a natural search task: Active object handling supports object location memory beyond the object’s identity , 2016, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[17]  Mary M. Hayhoe,et al.  Memory and visual search in naturalistic 2D and 3D environments , 2016, Journal of vision.

[18]  J. Hupé,et al.  Bistability for audiovisual stimuli: Perceptual decision is modality specific. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[19]  L. Maloney,et al.  Decision-theoretic models of visual perception and action , 2010, Vision Research.

[20]  J. Wolfe,et al.  When does repeated search in scenes involve memory? Looking at versus looking for objects in scenes. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  T. McNamara,et al.  Subjective hierarchies in spatial memory. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[22]  M. Land,et al.  The Roles of Vision and Eye Movements in the Control of Activities of Daily Living , 1998, Perception.

[23]  A. Hollingworth Object-position binding in visual memory for natural scenes and object arrays. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  N. Burgess,et al.  Spatial memory: how egocentric and allocentric combine , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  Mary Hayhoe,et al.  Visual Cognition , 2021, Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science.

[26]  Timothy F. Brady,et al.  Spatial constraints on learning in visual search: modeling contextual cuing. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  P. Glimcher,et al.  The Neurobiology of Decision: Consensus and Controversy , 2009, Neuron.

[28]  Elizabeth R. Chrastil,et al.  Active and passive contributions to spatial learning , 2011, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[29]  Yuhong V Jiang,et al.  Spatial reference frame of attention in a large outdoor environment. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  Manabu Iguchi,et al.  A bouncing droplet on an inclined hydrophobic surface , 2010, J. Vis..

[31]  Yuhong V. Jiang,et al.  What is learned in spatial contextual cuing— configuration or individual locations? , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.

[32]  J. Wolfe,et al.  The role of memory for visual search in scenes , 2015, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[33]  G. Christophides,et al.  Plasmodium berghei P47 is essential for ookinete protection from the Anopheles gambiae complement-like response , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[34]  D. Ballard,et al.  Memory Representations in Natural Tasks , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[35]  P. Glimcher Understanding dopamine and reinforcement learning: The dopamine reward prediction error hypothesis , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[36]  J. Hamill,et al.  Energetic Cost and Stability during Human Walking at the Preferred Stride Frequency , 1995 .

[37]  K. Fujii,et al.  Visualization for the analysis of fluid motion , 2005, J. Vis..

[38]  Todd S. Horowitz,et al.  Visual search has no memory , 1998, Nature.

[39]  A. Hollingworth Two forms of scene memory guide visual search: Memory for scene context and memory for the binding of target object to scene location , 2009 .

[40]  T. Foulsham,et al.  The where, what and when of gaze allocation in the lab and the natural environment , 2011, Vision Research.

[41]  J. Wolfe,et al.  The interplay of episodic and semantic memory in guiding repeated search in scenes , 2013, Cognition.

[42]  Andrew Hollingworth,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology : Human Perception and Performance The Nesting of Search Contexts Within Natural Scenes : Evidence From Contextual Cuing , 2010 .

[43]  D. Ballard,et al.  Task constraints in visual working memory , 1997, Vision Research.

[44]  Mary Hayhoe,et al.  Eye Movements, Visual Search and Scene Memory, in an Immersive Virtual Environment , 2014, PloS one.

[45]  Mary M Hayhoe,et al.  Vision in the natural world. , 2011, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[46]  Alan Kingstone,et al.  Balancing energetic and cognitive resources: Memory use during search depends on the orienting effector , 2014, Cognition.

[47]  Angela A. Manginelli,et al.  Misleading contextual cues: How do they affect visual search? , 2009, Psychological research.

[48]  Benjamin W Tatler,et al.  The influence of instructions on object memory in a real-world setting. , 2013, Journal of vision.

[49]  J. Oller,et al.  Consensus and controversy , 1984 .

[50]  Dejan Draschkow,et al.  Scene grammar shapes the way we interact with objects, strengthens memories, and speeds search , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[51]  M. Chun,et al.  Perceptual constraints on implicit learning of spatial context , 2002 .

[52]  Michael L. Platt,et al.  Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex , 1999, Nature.