Understanding young children's imitative behavior from an individual differences perspective

Research has shown that after observing a sequence of object-related actions, young children sometimes imitate the goal-directed aspects of the actions only, but other times faithfully imitate all aspects of the actions. In this study we explore whether this mixture of goal-directed and faithful imitation is based in part on individual differences between children. Forty-eight 2-year-old children (mean age = 26 months) completed a series of imitation tasks. Results revealed stable individual differences in children’s imitation—measurements of their imitative behavior correlated both within and between different types of imitation tasks. We further used Principle Component Analyses to cluster these correlated measurements into two factors, and the two factors aligned well with the concepts of goal-directed and faithful imitation.

[1]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Fourteen-through 18-month-old infants di eren-tially imitate intentional and accidental actions , 1998 .

[2]  B. Kenward,et al.  Over-imitation is better explained by norm learning than by distorted causal learning , 2011, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[3]  A. Meltzoff Understanding the Intentions of Others: Re-Enactment of Intended Acts by 18-Month-Old Children. , 1995, Developmental psychology.

[4]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Twelve- and 18-month-olds copy actions in terms of goals. , 2005, Developmental science.

[5]  Elma E. Hilbrink,et al.  Selective and faithful imitation at 12 and 15 months. , 2013, Developmental science.

[6]  A. Whiten,et al.  Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens) , 2005, Animal Cognition.

[7]  M. Nielsen,et al.  Copying actions and copying outcomes: social learning through the second year. , 2006, Developmental psychology.

[8]  H. Wellman,et al.  Infant attention to intentional action predicts preschool theory of mind. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[9]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Processes of social learning in the tool use of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens). , 1993, Journal of comparative psychology.

[10]  A. Lockerd,et al.  Understanding Intentions , 2022 .

[11]  Chris Moore,et al.  Young children overimitate in third-party contexts. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[12]  S. M. Carlson,et al.  Executive function and theory of mind: stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3. , 2004, Developmental psychology.

[13]  Andrew G. Young,et al.  The hidden structure of overimitation , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[14]  A. Woodward Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach , 1998, Cognition.

[15]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[16]  D. L. Mumme,et al.  Doing the right thing: infants' selection of actions to imitate from observed event sequences. , 2007, Child development.

[17]  M. Carpenter,et al.  Putting the social into social learning: explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children's copying behavior. , 2012, Journal of comparative psychology.

[18]  Yue Yu,et al.  It's all about the game: Infants' action strategies during imitation are influenced by their prior expectations , 2011, CogSci.

[19]  T. Kushnir,et al.  Social context effects in 2- and 4-year-olds' selective versus faithful imitation. , 2014, Developmental psychology.

[20]  Frank C Keil,et al.  The scope and limits of overimitation in the transmission of artefact culture , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[21]  Andrew Whiten,et al.  Imitation of causally opaque versus causally transparent tool use by 3- and 5-year-old children , 2007 .

[22]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Developmental psychology: Rational imitation in preverbal infants , 2002, Nature.

[23]  Andrew Whiten,et al.  Emulation and "overemulation" in the social learning of causally opaque versus causally transparent tool use by 23- and 30-month-olds. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.