Action Experience, More than Observation, Influences Mu Rhythm Desynchronization

Since the discovery of mirror neurons in premotor and parietal areas of the macaque monkey, the idea that action and perception may share the same neural code has been of central interest in social, developmental, and cognitive neurosciences. A fundamental question concerns how a putative human mirror neuron system may be tuned to the motor experiences of the individual. The current study tested the hypothesis that prior motor experience modulated the sensorimotor mu and beta rhythms. Specifically, we hypothesized that these sensorimotor rhythms would be more desynchronized after active motor experience compared to passive observation experience. To test our hypothesis, we collected EEG from adult participants during the observation of a relatively novel action: an experimenter used a claw-like tool to pick up a toy. Prior to EEG collection, we trained one group of adults to perform this action with the tool (performers). A second group comprised trained video coders, who only had experience observing the action (observers). Both the performers and the observers had no prior motor and visual experience with the action. A third group of novices was also tested. Performers exhibited the greatest mu rhythm desynchronization in the 8–13 Hz band, particularly in the right hemisphere compared to observers and novices. This study is the first to contrast active tool-use experience and observation experience in the mu rhythm and to show modulation with relatively shorter amounts of experience than prior mirror neuron expertise studies. These findings are discussed with respect to its broader implication as a neural signature for a mechanism of early social learning.

[1]  C. Keysers,et al.  μ-Suppression during Action Observation and Execution Correlates with BOLD in Dorsal Premotor, Inferior Parietal, and SI Cortices , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  Blake W. Johnson,et al.  Mu rhythm modulation during observation of an object-directed grasp. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[3]  N. Fox,et al.  The mirror mechanism and mu rhythm in social development , 2013, Neuroscience Letters.

[4]  Stavros M. Panas,et al.  Revealing Action Representation Processes in Audio Perception Using Fractal EEG Analysis , 2011, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[5]  Jessica A. Sommerville,et al.  Infants' Sensitivity to the Causal Features of Means‐End Support Sequences in Action and Perception , 2005 .

[6]  Lorna C. Quandt,et al.  Experience with novel actions modulates frontal alpha EEG desynchronization , 2011, Neuroscience Letters.

[7]  C. Moore,et al.  Intentional relations and social understanding , 1996, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[8]  Leonardo Fogassi,et al.  Mirror Neurons Responding to Observation of Actions Made with Tools in Monkey Ventral Premotor Cortex , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[9]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  Riitta Hari,et al.  Activation of the human primary motor cortex during observation of tool use , 2004, NeuroImage.

[11]  R. Passingham,et al.  Seeing or Doing? Influence of Visual and Motor Familiarity in Action Observation , 2006, Current Biology.

[12]  A. Meltzoff 1 Imitation and Other Minds: The "Like Me" Hypothesis , 2005 .

[13]  Lorna C. Quandt,et al.  Somatosensory experiences with action modulate alpha and beta power during subsequent action observation , 2013, Brain Research.

[14]  A. Woodward,et al.  Action production influences 12-month-old infants' attention to others' actions. , 2012, Developmental science.

[15]  M. Iacoboni,et al.  Movement-related desynchronization of alpha rhythms is lower in athletes than non-athletes: A high-resolution EEG study , 2010, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[16]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  Sensitivity of alpha and beta oscillations to sensorimotor characteristics of action: An EEG study of action production and gesture observation , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  P. L. Adams THE ORIGINS OF INTELLIGENCE IN CHILDREN , 1976 .

[18]  Claudio Del Percio,et al.  Pre-stimulus alpha rhythms are correlated with post-stimulus sensorimotor performance in athletes and non-athletes: A high-resolution EEG study , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[19]  A. Iriki,et al.  Mirror neurons through the lens of epigenetics , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[20]  J. Baldwin Social and Ethical Interpretations in Mental Development: A Study in Social Psychology , 1973 .

[21]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Neural mirroring systems: Exploring the EEG mu rhythm in human infancy , 2011, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[22]  F. L. D. Silva,et al.  Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[23]  T. Shipley,et al.  Effects of brief imitative experience on EEG desynchronization during action observation , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  Blake W. Johnson,et al.  Primary motor cortex activation during action observation revealed by wavelet analysis of the EEG , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[25]  David R. Olson,et al.  Developing theories of intention : social understanding and self-control , 1999 .

[26]  S Hurley,et al.  Perspectives on Imitation , 2004 .

[27]  A. Villringer,et al.  Rolandic alpha and beta EEG rhythms' strengths are inversely related to fMRI‐BOLD signal in primary somatosensory and motor cortex , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[28]  Hein T. van Schie,et al.  You'll never crawl alone: Neurophysiological evidence for experience-dependent motor resonance in infancy , 2008, NeuroImage.

[29]  M. Iacoboni,et al.  Golf putt outcomes are predicted by sensorimotor cerebral EEG rhythms , 2008, The Journal of physiology.

[30]  Olga Kochukhova,et al.  Goal anticipation during action observation is influenced by synonymous action capabilities, a puzzling developmental study , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  J. Wolpaw,et al.  Mu and Beta Rhythm Topographies During Motor Imagery and Actual Movements , 2004, Brain Topography.

[32]  Sarah A. Gerson,et al.  Learning from their own actions: the unique effect of producing actions on infants' action understanding. , 2014, Child development.

[33]  Richard Ramsey,et al.  Physical experience leads to enhanced object perception in parietal cortex: Insights from knot tying , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  Maria Alessandra Umiltà,et al.  Responses of mirror neurons in area F5 to hand and tool grasping observation , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  N. N. Rüther,et al.  Observed manipulation of novel tools leads to mu rhythm suppression over sensory-motor cortices , 2014, Behavioural Brain Research.

[36]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Lateralization of the Human Mirror Neuron System , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  Febo Cincotti,et al.  Human Cortical Electroencephalography (EEG) Rhythms during the Observation of Simple Aimless Movements: A High-Resolution EEG Study , 2002, NeuroImage.

[38]  A. Woodward The infant origins of intentional understanding. , 2005, Advances in child development and behavior.

[39]  L. Oberman,et al.  What goads cigarette smokers to smoke? Neural adaptation and the mirror neuron system , 2006, Brain Research.

[40]  J. Sommerville,et al.  Experience matters: the impact of doing versus watching on infants' subsequent perception of tool-use events. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[41]  R. Passingham,et al.  Action observation and acquired motor skills: an FMRI study with expert dancers. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[42]  M. Heil,et al.  Expertise in dance modulates alpha/beta event‐related desynchronization during action observation , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.

[43]  Amanda L. Woodward,et al.  Social Cognition and Social Responsiveness in 10-month-old Infants , 2007 .

[44]  G Rizzolatti,et al.  When pliers become fingers in the monkey motor system , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[45]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The Dynamics of Sensorimotor Cortical Oscillations during the Observation of Hand Movements: An EEG Study , 2012, PloS one.

[46]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[47]  Febo Cincotti,et al.  Human Movement-Related Potentials vs Desynchronization of EEG Alpha Rhythm: A High-Resolution EEG Study , 1999, NeuroImage.

[48]  Blake W. Johnson,et al.  Changes in rolandic mu rhythm during observation of a precision grip. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[49]  Lucy M. McGarry,et al.  Audio-visual facilitation of the mu rhythm , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[50]  M. Lassonde,et al.  Action related sounds induce early and late modulations of motor cortex activity , 2010, Neuroreport.

[51]  W. Ray,et al.  EEG alpha activity reflects attentional demands, and beta activity reflects emotional and cognitive processes. , 1985, Science.

[52]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Activation of human primary motor cortex during action observation: a neuromagnetic study. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[53]  G Pfurtscheller,et al.  Induced Oscillations in the Alpha Band: Functional Meaning , 2003, Epilepsia.

[54]  W. Klimesch EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis , 1999, Brain Research Reviews.

[55]  Emily S. Cross,et al.  Building a motor simulation de novo: Observation of dance by dancers , 2006, NeuroImage.

[56]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[57]  Harold Bekkering,et al.  Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience How Learning to Shake a Rattle Affects 8-month-old Infants' Perception of the Rattle's Sound: Electrophysiological Evidence for Action-effect Binding in Infancy , 2022 .

[58]  I. Gorodnitsky,et al.  EEG mu component responses to viewing emotional faces , 2012, Behavioural Brain Research.

[59]  Nadim Joni Shah,et al.  Prefrontal involvement in imitation learning of hand actions: Effects of practice and expertise , 2007, NeuroImage.

[60]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Parietal Lobe: From Action Organization to Intention Understanding , 2005, Science.

[61]  P. Holmes,et al.  Electroencephalographic functional equivalence during observation of action , 2006, Journal of sports sciences.

[62]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action recognition in the premotor cortex. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[63]  M. Tomasello Having intentions, understanding intentions, and understanding communicative intentions , 1999 .