Resting-State Subjective Experience and EEG Biomarkers Are Associated with Sleep-Onset Latency

Difficulties initiating sleep are common in several disorders, including insomnia and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. These disorders are prevalent, bearing significant societal and financial costs which require the consideration of new treatment strategies and a better understanding of the physiological and cognitive processes surrounding the time of preparing for sleep or falling asleep. Here, we search for neuro-cognitive associations in the resting state and examine their relevance for predicting sleep-onset latency using multi-level mixed models. Multiple EEG recordings were obtained from healthy male participants (N = 13) during a series of 5 min eyes-closed resting-state trials (in total, n = 223) followed by a period–varying in length up to 30 min–that either allowed subjects to transition into sleep (“sleep trials,” nsleep = 144) or was ended while they were still awake (“wake trials,” nwake = 79). After both eyes-closed rest, sleep and wake trials, subjective experience was assessed using the Amsterdam Resting-State Questionnaire (ARSQ). Our data revealed multiple associations between eyes-closed rest alpha and theta oscillations and ARSQ-dimensions Discontinuity of Mind, Self, Theory of Mind, Planning, and Sleepiness. The sleep trials showed that the transition toward the first sleep stage exclusively affected subjective experiences related to Theory of Mind, Planning, and Sleepiness. Importantly, sleep-onset latency was negatively associated both with eyes-closed rest ratings on the ARSQ dimension of Sleepiness and with the long-range temporal correlations of parietal theta oscillations derived by detrended fluctuation analysis (DFA). These results could be relevant to the development of personalized tools that help evaluate the success of falling asleep based on measures of resting-state cognition and EEG biomarkers.

[1]  T. Koenig,et al.  Brain electric microstates and momentary conscious mind states as building blocks of spontaneous thinking: I. Visual imagery and abstract thoughts. , 1998, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[2]  D Lehmann,et al.  Spontaneous conscious covert cognition states and brain electric spectral states in canonical correlations. , 1995, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[3]  R. B. Reilly,et al.  FASTER: Fully Automated Statistical Thresholding for EEG artifact Rejection , 2010, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[4]  J. Smallwood,et al.  The restless mind. , 2006, Psychological bulletin.

[5]  D. Posthuma,et al.  Sex differences in adults' motivation to achieve , 2010 .

[6]  E. Basar,et al.  Gamma, alpha, delta, and theta oscillations govern cognitive processes. , 2001, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[7]  A. Anastasi Individual differences. , 2020, Annual review of psychology.

[8]  Marc Joliot,et al.  The resting state questionnaire: An introspective questionnaire for evaluation of inner experience during the conscious resting state , 2010, Brain Research Bulletin.

[9]  Maurice M Ohayon,et al.  Epidemiology of insomnia: what we know and what we still need to learn. , 2002, Sleep medicine reviews.

[10]  A. Rechtschaffen,et al.  A manual of standardized terminology, technique and scoring system for sleep stages of human subjects , 1968 .

[11]  Canan Basar-Eroglu,et al.  Frontal theta activity is pronounced during illusory perception. , 2014, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[12]  J Aharon-Peretz,et al.  Impaired “Affective Theory of Mind” Is Associated with Right Ventromedial Prefrontal Damage , 2005, Cognitive and behavioral neurology : official journal of the Society for Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology.

[13]  Cornelis J. Stam,et al.  Magnetoencephalographic evaluation of resting-state functional connectivity in Alzheimer's disease , 2006, NeuroImage.

[14]  M. Raichle,et al.  Disease and the brain's dark energy , 2010, Nature Reviews Neurology.

[15]  D. Foulkes,et al.  Individual differences in mental activity at sleep onset. , 1966, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[16]  D. Bates,et al.  Linear Mixed-Effects Models using 'Eigen' and S4 , 2015 .

[17]  D L Schacter,et al.  The hypnagogic state: a critical review of the literature. , 1976, Psychological bulletin.

[18]  Benjamin W. Mooneyham,et al.  The Amsterdam Resting-State Questionnaire reveals multiple phenotypes of resting-state cognition , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[19]  Arjen van Ooyen,et al.  Altered temporal correlations in parietal alpha and prefrontal theta oscillations in early-stage Alzheimer disease , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[20]  C. Smyth,et al.  The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI). , 2000, Director.

[21]  Jennifer C. Britton,et al.  Linear mixed-effects modeling approach to FMRI group analysis , 2013, NeuroImage.

[22]  D. Gilbert,et al.  A Wandering Mind Is an Unhappy Mind , 2010, Science.

[23]  Lizeth H. Sloot,et al.  EEG-Biofeedback as a Tool to Modulate Arousal: Trends and Perspectives for Treatment of ADHD and Insomnia , 2012 .

[24]  A. Mognon,et al.  ADJUST: An automatic EEG artifact detector based on the joint use of spatial and temporal features. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[25]  M. Ferrara,et al.  Antero-posterior EEG changes during the wakefulness–sleep transition , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[26]  L Eugene Arnold,et al.  A Review of Neurofeedback Treatment for Pediatric ADHD , 2012, Journal of attention disorders.

[27]  Chien-Ming Yang,et al.  What subjective experiences determine the perception of falling asleep during sleep onset period? , 2010, Consciousness and Cognition.

[28]  A. Harvey,et al.  Catastrophic worry in primary insomnia. , 2003, Journal of behavior therapy and experimental psychiatry.

[29]  D. Halpern,et al.  The new science of cognitive sex differences , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[30]  Abraham Z. Snyder,et al.  A default mode of brain function: A brief history of an evolving idea , 2007, NeuroImage.

[31]  K. Greenwood,et al.  Identifying effective psychological treatments for insomnia: a meta-analysis. , 1995, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[32]  Søren Højsgaard,et al.  A Kenward-Roger approximation and parametric bootstrap methods for tests in linear mixed models: The R Package pbkrtest , 2014 .

[33]  C. Morin,et al.  Nonpharmacological interventions for insomnia: a meta-analysis of treatment efficacy. , 1994, The American journal of psychiatry.

[34]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[35]  P. Fries Neuronal gamma-band synchronization as a fundamental process in cortical computation. , 2009, Annual review of neuroscience.

[36]  Tadao Hori,et al.  Topographical EEG changes and the hypnagogic experience. , 1994 .

[37]  W. Dement,et al.  Awareness of sleepiness and ability to predict sleep onset: can drivers avoid falling asleep at the wheel? , 2007, Sleep medicine.

[38]  E. V. van Someren,et al.  Diminished capability to recognize the optimal temperature for sleep initiation may contribute to poor sleep in elderly people. , 2008, Sleep.

[39]  R. Nathan Spreng,et al.  Patterns of Brain Activity Supporting Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, and Theory of Mind, and Their Relationship to the Default Mode Network , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[40]  Dietrich Lehmann,et al.  Brain electrical activity and subjective experience during altered states of consciousness: ganzfeld and hypnagogic states. , 2002, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[41]  Marjorie Taylor,et al.  Neural Correlates of Theory-of-Mind Reasoning: An Event-Related Potential Study , 2000, Psychological science.

[42]  R. Xu Measuring explained variation in linear mixed effects models , 2003, Statistics in medicine.

[43]  B. Feige,et al.  The microstructure of sleep in primary insomnia: an overview and extension. , 2013, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[44]  T. Jaeger,et al.  Categorical Data Analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards Logit Mixed Models. , 2008, Journal of memory and language.

[45]  D. Foulkes,et al.  Mental activity in relaxed wakefulness. , 1975, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[46]  C. Morin,et al.  Insomnia and its relationship to health-care utilization, work absenteeism, productivity and accidents. , 2009, Sleep medicine.

[47]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[48]  C. Shapiro,et al.  Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) , 2011 .

[49]  Febo Cincotti,et al.  Out of the frying pan into the fire--the P300-based BCI faces real-world challenges. , 2011, Progress in brain research.

[50]  K. Linkenkaer-Hansen,et al.  Long-Range Temporal Correlations in Resting-State Alpha Oscillations Predict Human Timing-Error Dynamics , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  Diederick Stoffers,et al.  The ARSQ 2.0 reveals age and personality effects on mind-wandering experiences , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[52]  L. Creti,et al.  A COGNITIVE MODEL OF INSOMNIA , 2022 .

[53]  P. Uhlhaas,et al.  Working memory and neural oscillations: alpha–gamma versus theta–gamma codes for distinct WM information? , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[54]  Matthijs Verhage,et al.  A solution to dependency: using multilevel analysis to accommodate nested data , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.

[55]  Uwe Friese,et al.  Theta–gamma coupling during episodic retrieval in the human EEG , 2014, Brain Research.

[56]  H. Bergh,et al.  Examples of Mixed-Effects Modeling with Crossed Random Effects and with Binomial Data. , 2008 .

[57]  Kai Spiegelhalder,et al.  Increased EEG sigma and beta power during NREM sleep in primary insomnia , 2012, Biological Psychology.

[58]  F. Kruggel,et al.  Theta-power Differences in Patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment Under Rest Condition and During Haptic Tasks , 2002, Alzheimer disease and associated disorders.

[59]  M. Kane,et al.  Does mind wandering reflect executive function or executive failure? Comment on Smallwood and Schooler (2006) and Watkins (2008). , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[60]  M. Raichle The Brain's Dark Energy , 2006, Science.

[61]  J Leon Kenemans,et al.  Resting-state EEG theta activity and risk learning: sensitivity to reward or punishment? , 2014, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[62]  Kai Spiegelhalder,et al.  Sleep changes in the disorder of insomnia: a meta-analysis of polysomnographic studies. , 2014, Sleep medicine reviews.

[63]  Klaus Linkenkaer-Hansen,et al.  Breakdown of Long-Range Temporal Correlations in Theta Oscillations in Patients with Major Depressive Disorder , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[64]  Ana Ivelisse Avilés,et al.  Linear Mixed Models for Longitudinal Data , 2001, Technometrics.

[65]  Martijn van de Pol,et al.  A simple method for distinguishing within- versus between-subject effects using mixed models , 2009, Animal Behaviour.

[66]  P. Achermann,et al.  Fronto‐occipital EEG power gradients in human sleep , 1997, Journal of sleep research.

[67]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[68]  Mollie E. Brooks,et al.  Generalized linear mixed models: a practical guide for ecology and evolution. , 2009, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[69]  E. V. van Someren,et al.  Effect of melatonin on sleep, behavior, and cognition in ADHD and chronic sleep-onset insomnia. , 2007, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[70]  Daniel J Buysse,et al.  The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research , 1989, Psychiatry Research.

[71]  W. Klimesch,et al.  Control mechanisms in working memory: A possible function of EEG theta oscillations , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[72]  R. Nathan Spreng,et al.  The Common Neural Basis of Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, Navigation, Theory of Mind, and the Default Mode: A Quantitative Meta-analysis , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[73]  Vadim V. Nikulin,et al.  Detrended Fluctuation Analysis: A Scale-Free View on Neuronal Oscillations , 2012, Front. Physio..

[74]  Leanne M Williams,et al.  Resting EEG theta activity predicts cognitive performance in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. , 2005, Pediatric neurology.

[75]  H. Flor,et al.  A spelling device for the paralysed , 1999, Nature.

[76]  M. Hayashi,et al.  Topographic mapping of electroencephalography coherence in hypnagogic state , 1998, Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences.