Infant contributions to joint attention predict vocabulary development

Joint attention has long been accepted as constituting a privileged circumstance in which word learning prospers. Consequently research has investigated the role that maternal responsiveness to infant attention plays in predicting language outcomes. However there has been a recent expansion in research implicating similar predictive effects from individual differences in infant behaviours. Emerging from the foundations of such work comes an interesting question: do the relative contributions of the mother and infant to joint attention episodes impact upon language learning? In an attempt to address this, two joint attention behaviours were assessed as predictors of vocabulary attainment (as measured by OCDI Production Scores). These predictors were: mothers encouraging attention to an object given their infant was already attending to an object (maternal follow-in); and infants looking to an object given their mothers encouragement of attention to an object (infant follow-in). In a sample of 14- month old children (N=36) we compared the predictive power of these maternal and infant follow-in variables on concurrent and later language performance. Results using Growth Curve Analysis provided evidence that while both maternal follow-in and infant follow-in variables contributed to production scores, infant follow-in was a stronger predictor. Consequently it does appear to matter whose final contribution establishes joint attention episodes. Infants who more often follow-in into their mothers’ encouragement of attention have larger, and faster growing vocabularies between 14 and 18-months of age.

[1]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Infants' contribution to the achievement of joint reference. , 1991, Child development.

[2]  D. Mirman,et al.  Statistical and computational models of the visual world paradigm: Growth curves and individual differences. , 2008, Journal of memory and language.

[3]  Michael Morales,et al.  Responding to Joint Attention Across the 6- Through 24-Month Age Period and Early Language Acquisition , 2000 .

[4]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Visual Attention Is Not Enough: Individual Differences in Statistical Word-Referent Learning in Infants , 2013, Language learning and development : the official journal of the Society for Language Development.

[5]  S. Waxman,et al.  Early word-learning entails reference, not merely associations , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  Michael Morales,et al.  Individual Differences in Infant Skills as Predictors of Child-Caregiver Joint Attention and Language , 2000 .

[7]  Habituation and maternal encouragement of attention in infancy as predictors of toddler language, play, and representational competence. , 1989 .

[8]  M. Bornstein,et al.  Maternal responsiveness and children's achievement of language milestones. , 2001, Child development.

[9]  P. Mundy,et al.  CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Attention, Joint Attention, and Social Cognition , 2022 .

[10]  Valerie Flynn,et al.  Maternal responsive and directive behaviours and utterances as predictors of children's lexical development , 2005, Journal of Child Language.

[11]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Not your mother's view: the dynamics of toddler visual experience. , 2011, Developmental science.

[12]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. , 1998, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[13]  K Plunkett,et al.  Infant vocabulary development assessed with a British communicative development inventory , 2000, Journal of Child Language.

[14]  P. Dunham,et al.  Directive interactions and early vocabulary development: the role of joint attentional focus , 1991, Journal of Child Language.

[15]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Joint attention and early language. , 1986, Child development.

[16]  Elma E. Hilbrink,et al.  CUE: The continuous unified electronic diary method , 2012, Behavior Research Methods.

[17]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Infants' ability to consult the speaker for clues to word reference , 1993, Journal of Child Language.

[18]  M. Bornstein,et al.  Habituation and maternal encouragement of attention in infancy as predictors of toddler language, play, and representational competence. , 1989, Child development.

[19]  Andrew N Meltzoff,et al.  Infant gaze following and pointing predict accelerated vocabulary growth through two years of age: a longitudinal, growth curve modeling study* , 2008, Journal of Child Language.

[20]  R. Bakeman,et al.  Sequential analysis and observational methods for the behavioral sciences , 2011 .

[21]  W. Dixon,et al.  Attentional Focus Moderates Habituation-Language Relationships: Slow Habituation May be a Good Thing , 2008 .

[22]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  The development of gaze following and its relation to language. , 2005, Developmental science.

[23]  P. Dale,et al.  Do Early Talkers Become Early Readers ? Linguistic Precocity , Preschool Language , and Emergent Literacy , 2004 .

[24]  Chen Yu,et al.  Word Learning through Sensorimotor Child-Parent Interaction: A Feature Selection Approach , 2011, CogSci.