Neural Tuning Size in a Model of Primate Visual Processing Accounts for Three Key Markers of Holistic Face Processing

Faces are an important and unique class of visual stimuli, and have been of interest to neuroscientists for many years. Faces are known to elicit certain characteristic behavioral markers, collectively labeled “holistic processing”, while non-face objects are not processed holistically. However, little is known about the underlying neural mechanisms. The main aim of this computational simulation work is to investigate the neural mechanisms that make face processing holistic. Using a model of primate visual processing, we show that a single key factor, “neural tuning size”, is able to account for three important markers of holistic face processing: the Composite Face Effect (CFE), Face Inversion Effect (FIE) and Whole-Part Effect (WPE). Our proof-of-principle specifies the precise neurophysiological property that corresponds to the poorly-understood notion of holism, and shows that this one neural property controls three classic behavioral markers of holism. Our work is consistent with neurophysiological evidence, and makes further testable predictions. Overall, we provide a parsimonious account of holistic face processing, connecting computation, behavior and neurophysiology.

[1]  R. Desimone,et al.  Stimulus-selective properties of inferior temporal neurons in the macaque , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[2]  Rachel A Robbins,et al.  A Review and Clarification of the Terms “holistic,” “configural,” and “relational” in the Face Perception Literature , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[3]  M. Farah,et al.  What is "special" about face perception? , 1998, Psychological review.

[4]  E. Rolls,et al.  INVARIANT FACE AND OBJECT RECOGNITION IN THE VISUAL SYSTEM , 1997, Progress in Neurobiology.

[5]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Oxford Handbook of Face Perception , 2011 .

[6]  H. Bülthoff,et al.  Face recognition under varying poses: The role of texture and shape , 1996, Vision Research.

[7]  Michael C. Mangini,et al.  Making the ineffable explicit: estimating the information employed for face classifications , 2004, Cogn. Sci..

[8]  Isabel Gauthier,et al.  Should we reject the expertise hypothesis? , 2007, Cognition.

[9]  Paul A. Viola,et al.  Robust Real-Time Face Detection , 2001, International Journal of Computer Vision.

[10]  Doris Y. Tsao,et al.  A face feature space in the macaque temporal lobe , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[11]  David G. Lowe,et al.  University of British Columbia. , 1945, Canadian Medical Association journal.

[12]  Michel Vidal-Naquet,et al.  Visual features of intermediate complexity and their use in classification , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  Thomas Serre,et al.  A feedforward architecture accounts for rapid categorization , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[14]  Reza Ebrahimpour,et al.  A specialized face-processing network consistent with the representational geometry of monkey face patches , 2015, ArXiv.

[15]  I. Gauthier,et al.  How does the brain process upright and inverted faces? , 2002, Behavioral and cognitive neuroscience reviews.

[16]  Rachel A Robbins,et al.  No face-like processing for objects-of-expertise in three behavioural tasks , 2007, Cognition.

[17]  N. Logothetis,et al.  IS FACE RECOGNITION NOT SO UNIQUE AFTER ALL? , 2000, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[18]  Kunihiko Fukushima,et al.  Neocognitron: A self-organizing neural network model for a mechanism of pattern recognition unaffected by shift in position , 1980, Biological Cybernetics.

[19]  M. Farah,et al.  Parts and Wholes in Face Recognition , 1993, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[20]  I Biederman,et al.  Neurocomputational bases of object and face recognition. , 1997, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[21]  Doris Y. Tsao,et al.  A Cortical Region Consisting Entirely of Face-Selective Cells , 2006, Science.

[22]  A. Young,et al.  Configurational Information in Face Perception , 1987, Perception.

[23]  James W. Tanaka,et al.  Features, Configuration, and Holistic Face Processing , 2011 .

[24]  David I. Perrett,et al.  Neurophysiology of shape processing , 1993, Image Vis. Comput..

[25]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Neural Basis of the Behavioral Face-Inversion Effect , 2005, Current Biology.

[26]  T. Poggio,et al.  Hierarchical models of object recognition in cortex , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[27]  Jennifer J. Richler,et al.  Meanings, Mechanisms, and Measures of Holistic Processing , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[28]  A. Young,et al.  ARE FACES SPECIAL , 1989 .

[29]  Garrison W. Cottrell,et al.  Do Expression and Identity Need Separate Representations? , 2019, Proceedings of the Twenty-Fourth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.

[30]  Doris Y. Tsao,et al.  Mechanisms of face perception. , 2008, Annual review of neuroscience.

[31]  D. Perrett,et al.  Visual neurones responsive to faces in the monkey temporal cortex , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  M. Riesenhuber,et al.  Task effects, performance levels, features, configurations, and holistic face processing: a reply to Rossion. , 2009, Acta psychologica.

[33]  D. Maurer,et al.  The many faces of configural processing , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[34]  Irving Biederman,et al.  A neurocomputational account of the face configural effect. , 2014, Journal of vision.

[35]  R. Yin Looking at Upside-down Faces , 1969 .