Limits on visual awareness of object targets in the context of other object category masks: Investigating bottlenecks in the continuous flash suppression paradigm with hand and tool stimuli.

The continuous flash suppression (CFS) task can be used to investigate what limits our capacity to become aware of visual stimuli. In this task, a stream of rapidly changing mask images to one eye initially suppresses awareness for a static target image presented to the other eye. Several factors may determine the breakthrough time from mask suppression, one of which is the overlap in representation of the target/mask categories in higher visual cortex. This hypothesis is based on certain object categories (e.g., faces) being more effective in blocking awareness of other categories (e.g., buildings) than other combinations (e.g., cars/chairs). Previous work found mask effectiveness to be correlated with category-pair high-level representational similarity. As the cortical representations of hands and tools overlap, these categories are ideal to test this further as well as to examine alternative explanations. For our CFS experiments, we predicted longer breakthrough times for hands/tools compared to other pairs due to the reported cortical overlap. In contrast, across three experiments, participants were generally faster at detecting targets masked by hands or tools compared to other mask categories. Exploring low-level explanations, we found that the category average for edges (e.g., hands have less detail compared to cars) was the best predictor for the data. This low-level bottleneck could not completely account for the specific category patterns and the hand/tool effects, suggesting there are several levels at which object category-specific limits occur. Given these findings, it is important that low-level bottlenecks for visual awareness are considered when testing higher-level hypotheses.

[1]  Ken Nakayama,et al.  Visual search for object categories is predicted by the representational architecture of high-level visual cortex. , 2017, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex , 1962, The Journal of physiology.

[3]  R. Blake,et al.  Neural bases of binocular rivalry , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[4]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[5]  Randolph Blake,et al.  Psychophysical magic: rendering the visible ‘invisible’ , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  Greg O. Horne,et al.  Controlling low-level image properties: The SHINE toolbox , 2010, Behavior research methods.

[7]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Unconscious processing dissociates along categorical lines , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[8]  M. Brodeur,et al.  The Bank of Standardized Stimuli (BOSS), a New Set of 480 Normative Photos of Objects to Be Used as Visual Stimuli in Cognitive Research , 2010, PloS one.

[9]  Susan L. Franzel,et al.  Guided search: an alternative to the feature integration model for visual search. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  Michael A. Cohen,et al.  What is the Bandwidth of Perceptual Experience? , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[11]  C. Koch,et al.  Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[12]  Jan Drewes,et al.  The edge of awareness: Mask spatial density, but not color, determines optimal temporal frequency for continuous flash suppression. , 2018, Journal of vision.

[13]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[14]  Li Su,et al.  A Toolbox for Representational Similarity Analysis , 2014, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[15]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Closely overlapping responses to tools and hands in left lateral occipitotemporal cortex. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  Wayne D. Gray,et al.  Basic objects in natural categories , 1976, Cognitive Psychology.

[17]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[18]  P. Sterzer,et al.  Investigating category‐ and shape‐selective neural processing in ventral and dorsal visual stream under interocular suppression , 2015, Human brain mapping.

[19]  Randolph Blake,et al.  Depth of interocular suppression associated with continuous flash suppression, flash suppression, and binocular rivalry. , 2006, Journal of vision.

[20]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[21]  Michael A. Cohen,et al.  Processing multiple visual objects is limited by overlap in neural channels , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[22]  G. Smyth,et al.  Statistical Applications in Genetics and Molecular Biology Permutation P -values Should Never Be Zero: Calculating Exact P -values When Permutations Are Randomly Drawn , 2011 .

[23]  Seongho Kim ppcor: An R Package for a Fast Calculation to Semi-partial Correlation Coefficients. , 2015, Communications for statistical applications and methods.

[24]  P. Haggard,et al.  Sense of Agency Primes Manual Motor Responses , 2009, Perception.

[25]  M. Eimer,et al.  Response Facilitation and Inhibition in Manual, Vocal, and Oculomotor Performance: Evidence for a Modality-Unspecific Mechanism , 2001, Journal of motor behavior.

[26]  A. Oliva,et al.  A Real-World Size Organization of Object Responses in Occipitotemporal Cortex , 2012, Neuron.

[27]  Philipp Sterzer,et al.  Priming in a shape task but not in a category task under continuous flash suppression. , 2016, Journal of vision.

[28]  Marius V. Peelen,et al.  Privileged detection of conspecifics: Evidence from inversion effects during continuous flash suppression , 2012, Cognition.

[29]  George A. Alvarez,et al.  Visual Awareness Is Limited by the Representational Architecture of the Visual System , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[30]  D. Melcher,et al.  Time for Awareness: The Influence of Temporal Properties of the Mask on Continuous Flash Suppression Effectiveness , 2016, PloS one.

[31]  John F. Canny,et al.  A Computational Approach to Edge Detection , 1986, IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence.

[32]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Matching Categorical Object Representations in Inferior Temporal Cortex of Man and Monkey , 2008, Neuron.

[33]  W. Miles Ocular dominance in human adults. , 1930 .