Perception of dynamic Glass patterns
暂无分享,去创建一个
Christopher R. Madan | Jean-François Nankoo | Marcia L. Spetch | Douglas R. Wylie | M. Spetch | C. Madan | J. Nankoo | D. Wylie
[1] Alan L. F. Lee,et al. A comparison of global motion perception using a multiple-aperture stimulus. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[2] T. Poggio,et al. Cognitive neuroscience: Neural mechanisms for the recognition of biological movements , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[3] Randolph Blake,et al. Detection and discrimination of optical flow components , 1998 .
[4] R. M. Siegel,et al. Perception of three-dimensional structure from motion in monkey and man , 1988, Nature.
[5] Eli Brenner,et al. Flexibility in intercepting moving objects. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[6] D. Burr,et al. Cardinal directions for visual optic flow , 1999, Current Biology.
[7] Frank Bremmer,et al. Neural correlates of implied motion , 2003, Nature.
[8] J. Gallant,et al. A Human Extrastriate Area Functionally Homologous to Macaque V4 , 2000, Neuron.
[9] Stephen J Anderson,et al. Neuroimaging in Human Amblyopia , 2006, Strabismus.
[10] Stephen J. Anderson,et al. MEG Responses to the Perception of Global Structure within Glass Patterns , 2010, PloS one.
[11] Gunter Loffler,et al. An inverse oblique effect in human vision , 2001, Vision Research.
[12] K. Grill-Spector,et al. Repetition and the brain: neural models of stimulus-specific effects , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[13] H. Wilson,et al. Detection of global structure in Glass patterns: implications for form vision , 1998, Vision Research.
[14] Francesca Pei,et al. Event-related potentials show configural specificity of global form processing , 2005, Neuroreport.
[15] Z. Kourtzi,et al. Linking form and motion in the primate brain , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[16] D. Hubel,et al. Segregation of form, color, movement, and depth: anatomy, physiology, and perception. , 1988, Science.
[17] J. Movshon,et al. Glass pattern responses in macaque V2 neurons. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[18] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical pathways , 1983, Trends in Neurosciences.
[19] Edward A Essock,et al. A horizontal bias in human visual processing of orientation and its correspondence to the structural components of natural scenes. , 2004, Journal of vision.
[20] D. Burr,et al. Two stages of visual processing for radial and circular motion , 1995, Nature.
[21] K. Nakayama,et al. Intact “biological motion” and “structure from motion” perception in a patient with impaired motion mechanisms: A case study , 1990, Visual Neuroscience.
[22] Oliver Braddick,et al. Interaction of spatial and temporal integration in global form processing , 2006, Vision Research.
[23] Vincent P Ferrera,et al. Detection thresholds for spiral Glass patterns , 2001, Vision Research.
[24] R. Henson. Neuroimaging studies of priming , 2003, Progress in Neurobiology.
[25] Janette Atkinson,et al. Differential human brain activation by vertical and horizontal global visual textures , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[26] D. M. Green,et al. Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .
[27] Anthony J. Movshon,et al. Signals in Macaque Striate Cortical Neurons that Support the Perception of Glass Patterns , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[28] J A Beintema,et al. Perception of biological motion without local image motion , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[29] Wilson S. Geisler,et al. Motion streaks provide a spatial code for motion direction , 1999, Nature.
[30] Roberto Cipolla,et al. Structure from motion , 2008 .
[31] O. Braddick,et al. What is Noise for the Motion System? , 1996, Vision Research.
[32] I. Biederman,et al. Priming contour-deleted images: Evidence for intermediate representations in visual object recognition , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.
[33] Sieu K. Khuu,et al. The role of luminance contrast in the detection of global structure in static and dynamic, same- and opposite-polarity, Glass patterns , 2007, Vision Research.
[34] Leslie G. Ungerleider. Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .
[35] P. McLeod,et al. Preserved and Impaired Detection of Structure From Motion by a 'Motion-blind" Patient , 1996 .
[36] John A. Nelder,et al. A Simplex Method for Function Minimization , 1965, Comput. J..
[37] Zoe Kourtzi,et al. Implied motion from form in the human visual cortex. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.
[38] W. Weibull. A Statistical Distribution Function of Wide Applicability , 1951 .
[39] D. Burr,et al. The effects of opposite-polarity dipoles on the detection of Glass patterns , 2006, Vision Research.
[40] N. Mai,et al. Selective disturbance of movement vision after bilateral brain damage. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[41] S. C Dakin,et al. Summation of concentric orientation structure: seeing the Glass or the window? , 2002, Vision Research.
[42] R. Turner,et al. Form and motion coherence activate independent, but not dorsal/ventral segregated, networks in the human brain , 2000, Current Biology.
[43] W F Bischof,et al. Detection of Glass Patterns by Pigeons and Humans: Implications for Differences in Higher-Level Processing , 2001, Psychological science.
[44] Z. Kourtzi,et al. Neural coding of global form in the human visual cortex. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.
[45] M. Goodale,et al. The visual brain in action , 1995 .
[46] L. Glass. Moiré Effect from Random Dots , 1969, Nature.
[47] Alex Martin,et al. Properties and mechanisms of perceptual priming , 1998, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[48] David R. Badcock,et al. Coherent global motion in the absence of coherent velocity signals , 2000, Current Biology.
[49] Edward A. Essock,et al. Oblique stimuli are seen best (not worst!) in naturalistic broad-band stimuli: a horizontal effect , 2003, Vision Research.