Comparing Pupil Light Response Modulation between Saccade Planning and Working Memory

The signature of spatial attention effects has been demonstrated through saccade planning and working memory. Although saccade planning and working memory have been commonly linked to attention, the comparison of effects resulting from saccade planning and working memory is less explored. It has recently been shown that spatial attention interacts with local luminance at the attended location. When bright and dark patch stimuli are presented simultaneously in the periphery, thereby producing no change in global luminance, pupil size is nonetheless smaller when the locus of attention overlaps with the bright, compared to the dark patch stimulus (referred to as the local luminance modulation). Here, we used the local luminance modulation to directly compare the effects of saccade planning and spatial working memory. Participants were required to make a saccade towards a visual target location (visual-delay) or a remembered target location (memory-delay) after a variable delay, and the bright and dark patch stimuli were presented during the delay period between target onset and go signal. Greater pupil constriction was observed when the bright patch, compared to the dark patch, was spatially aligned with the target location in both tasks. However, the effects were diminished when there was no contingency implemented between the patch and target locations, particularly in the memory-delay task. Together, our results suggest the involvement of similar, but not identical, attentional mechanisms through saccade planning and working memory, and highlight a promising potential of local pupil luminance responses for probing visuospatial processing.

[1]  D. Munoz,et al.  Overt Responses during Covert Orienting , 2014, Neuron.

[2]  N. J. Gandhi,et al.  Motor functions of the superior colliculus. , 2011, Annual review of neuroscience.

[3]  M. Carrasco Visual attention: The past 25 years , 2011, Vision Research.

[4]  D. Munoz,et al.  Competitive Integration of Visual and Preparatory Signals in the Superior Colliculus during Saccadic Programming , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  A visual salience map in the primate frontal eye field. , 2005, Progress in brain research.

[6]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  The pupillary light response reflects exogenous attention and inhibition of return. , 2014, Journal of vision.

[7]  Okihide Hikosaka,et al.  Tonic activity during visuo-oculomotor behavior in the monkey superior colliculus , 1996, Neuroscience Research.

[8]  Gunnar Blohm,et al.  Multisensory integration in orienting behavior: Pupil size, microsaccades, and saccades , 2017, Biological Psychology.

[9]  F. Vitu,et al.  The pupillary light response reflects eye-movement preparation , 2014 .

[10]  R. Krauzlis,et al.  Superior colliculus and visual spatial attention. , 2013, Annual review of neuroscience.

[11]  J. Grainger,et al.  The Pupillary Light Response Reveals the Focus of Covert Visual Attention , 2013, PloS one.

[12]  Robert M. McPeek,et al.  Deficits in saccade target selection after inactivation of superior colliculus , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  Paola Binda,et al.  Spatial attention increases the pupillary response to light changes. , 2015, Journal of vision.

[14]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Progression in neuronal processing for saccadic eye movements from parietal cortex area lip to superior colliculus. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  Clayton E. Curtis,et al.  Persistent neural activity during the maintenance of spatial position in working memory , 2008, NeuroImage.

[16]  Stefan Everling,et al.  Task-relevant Output Signals are Sent from Monkey Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex to the Superior Colliculus during a Visuospatial Working Memory Task , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  D. Melcher,et al.  The Role of Attentional Priority and Saliency in Determining Capacity Limits in Enumeration and Visual Working Memory , 2011, PloS one.

[18]  D. Munoz,et al.  Neuronal Activity in Monkey Superior Colliculus Related to the Initiation of Saccadic Eye Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  J. Bisley The neural basis of visual attention , 2011, The Journal of physiology.

[20]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Composition and topographic organization of signals sent from the frontal eye field to the superior colliculus. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  Robert C. Wilson,et al.  Rational regulation of learning dynamics by pupil–linked arousal systems , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[22]  Brian J. White,et al.  Microstimulation of the Monkey Superior Colliculus Induces Pupil Dilation Without Evoking Saccades , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Role of the basal ganglia in the control of purposive saccadic eye movements. , 2000, Physiological reviews.

[24]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[25]  J. Beatty Task-evoked pupillary responses, processing load, and the structure of processing resources. , 1982, Psychological bulletin.

[26]  C. Cierpka,et al.  Particle imaging techniques for volumetric three-component (3D3C) velocity measurements in microfluidics , 2011, Journal of Visualization.

[27]  Paola Binda,et al.  Attention to Bright Surfaces Enhances the Pupillary Light Reflex , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  D. Munoz,et al.  Saccadic Probability Influences Motor Preparation Signals and Time to Saccadic Initiation , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  Etienne Olivier,et al.  Contribution of the Monkey Frontal Eye Field to Covert Visual Attention , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  A. King,et al.  The superior colliculus , 2004, Current Biology.

[31]  Sebastiaan Mathôt,et al.  New Light on the Mind’s Eye , 2015, Current directions in psychological science.

[32]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The effects of neural gain on attention and learning , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[33]  J. Gold,et al.  Relationships between Pupil Diameter and Neuronal Activity in the Locus Coeruleus, Colliculi, and Cingulate Cortex , 2016, Neuron.

[34]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. , 2005, Annual review of neuroscience.

[35]  George H. Denfield,et al.  Pupil Fluctuations Track Fast Switching of Cortical States during Quiet Wakefulness , 2014, Neuron.

[36]  Heiner Deubel,et al.  Oculomotor selection underlies feature retention in visual working memory. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  P. Binda,et al.  Keeping a large-pupilled eye on high-level visual processing , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[38]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Attention, intention, and priority in the parietal lobe. , 2010, Annual review of neuroscience.

[39]  Veit Stuphorn,et al.  Role of supplementary eye field in saccade initiation: executive, not direct, control. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  S. Laughlin Retinal information capacity and the function of the pupil , 1992, Ophthalmic & physiological optics : the journal of the British College of Ophthalmic Opticians.

[41]  S. van der Stigchel,et al.  Focus of spatial attention during spatial working memory maintenance: Evidence from pupillary light response , 2017 .

[42]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Monkey posterior parietal cortex neurons antidromically activated from superior colliculus. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Neuronal Mechanisms of Visual Attention. , 2015, Annual review of vision science.

[44]  Anne E. Urai,et al.  Pupil-linked arousal is driven by decision uncertainty and alters serial choice bias , 2017, Nature Communications.

[45]  Laurent Itti,et al.  Transient Pupil Response Is Modulated by Contrast-Based Saliency , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[46]  P. Roelfsema,et al.  Different States in Visual Working Memory: When It Guides Attention and When It Does Not , 2022 .

[47]  Clayton E Curtis,et al.  Prioritized Maps of Space in Human Frontoparietal Cortex , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  D. Munoz,et al.  Modulation of stimulus contrast on the human pupil orienting response , 2014, The European journal of neuroscience.

[49]  Katherine M. Armstrong,et al.  Visual and oculomotor selection: links, causes and implications for spatial attention , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[50]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[51]  N. Unsworth,et al.  Pupillary correlates of covert shifts of attention during working memory maintenance , 2017, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[52]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[53]  T. Knapen,et al.  Decision-related pupil dilation reflects upcoming choice and individual bias , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[54]  P. Binda,et al.  Pupil size reflects the focus of feature-based attention. , 2014, Journal of neurophysiology.

[55]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Signal transformations from cerebral cortex to superior colliculus for the generation of saccades , 2001, Vision Research.

[56]  T. Ichida World Medical Association declaration of Helsinki , 1991, Gastroenterologia Japonica.

[57]  W. C. Hall,et al.  The Superior Colliculus : New Approaches for Studying Sensorimotor Integration , 2003 .

[58]  Eileen Kowler Eye movements: The past 25years , 2011, Vision Research.

[59]  Alexandre Zénon,et al.  Attention deficits without cortical neuronal deficits , 2012, Nature.

[60]  G. Pari,et al.  Disruption of pupil size modulation correlates with voluntary motor preparation deficits in Parkinson's disease , 2016, Neuropsychologia.

[61]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Automatic guidance of attention from working memory , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[62]  Christiane,et al.  World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. , 2004, Journal international de bioethique = International journal of bioethics.

[63]  N. Unsworth,et al.  Tracking working memory maintenance with pupillometry , 2017, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[64]  J. Jonides,et al.  Overlapping mechanisms of attention and spatial working memory , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[65]  Tonya White,et al.  Oculomotor and pupillometric indices of pro- and antisaccade performance in youth-onset psychosis and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. , 2010, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[66]  Etienne Olivier,et al.  A Deficit in Covert Attention after Parietal Cortex Inactivation in the Monkey , 2004, Neuron.

[67]  Douglas P Munoz,et al.  A circuit for pupil orienting responses: implications for cognitive modulation of pupil size , 2015, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[68]  Richard J Krauzlis,et al.  Inactivation of primate superior colliculus impairs covert selection of signals for perceptual judgments , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[69]  J. M. Woodhouse,et al.  The effect of pupil size on grating detection at various contrast levels , 1975, Vision Research.

[70]  David L. Sparks,et al.  Movement selection in advance of action in the superior colliculus , 1992, Nature.

[71]  C. Curtis,et al.  Common neural mechanisms supporting spatial working memory, attention and motor intention , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[72]  Martin Rolfs,et al.  Saccadic Eye Movements Impose a Natural Bottleneck on Visual Short-Term Memory , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[73]  Manabu Iguchi,et al.  Entry of inclined hydrophobic and hydrophilic circular cylinders into water , 2011, J. Vis..

[74]  R. B. Ebitz,et al.  Neuronal Activity in Primate Dorsal Anterior Cingulate Cortex Signals Task Conflict and Predicts Adjustments in Pupil-Linked Arousal , 2015, Neuron.