Changes in cognitive flexibility and hypothesis search across human life history from childhood to adolescence to adulthood

How was the evolution of our unique biological life history related to distinctive human developments in cognition and culture? We suggest that the extended human childhood and adolescence allows a balance between exploration and exploitation, between wider and narrower hypothesis search, and between innovation and imitation in cultural learning. In particular, different developmental periods may be associated with different learning strategies. This relation between biology and culture was probably coevolutionary and bidirectional: life-history changes allowed changes in learning, which in turn both allowed and rewarded extended life histories. In two studies, we test how easily people learn an unusual physical or social causal relation from a pattern of evidence. We track the development of this ability from early childhood through adolescence and adulthood. In the physical domain, preschoolers, counterintuitively, perform better than school-aged children, who in turn perform better than adolescents and adults. As they grow older learners are less flexible: they are less likely to adopt an initially unfamiliar hypothesis that is consistent with new evidence. Instead, learners prefer a familiar hypothesis that is less consistent with the evidence. In the social domain, both preschoolers and adolescents are actually the most flexible learners, adopting an unusual hypothesis more easily than either 6-y-olds or adults. There may be important developmental transitions in flexibility at the entry into middle childhood and in adolescence, which differ across domains.

[1]  Illtyd Trethowan Causality , 1938 .

[2]  V. A. Harris,et al.  The Attribution of Attitudes , 1967 .

[3]  H. Kelley Attribution theory in social psychology , 1967 .

[4]  J. Bruner Nature and uses of immaturity. , 1972 .

[5]  C. D. Gelatt,et al.  Optimization by Simulated Annealing , 1983, Science.

[6]  R. Byrne,et al.  Machiavellian intelligence : social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans , 1990 .

[7]  P. Huttenlocher Morphometric study of human cerebral cortex development , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  B. L. Green,et al.  The Adaptive Nature of Cognitive Immaturity. , 1992 .

[9]  R. Byrne The Thinking Ape : Evolutionary Origins of Intelligence , 1995 .

[10]  Robin I. M. Dunbar The thinking ape: Evolutionary origins of intelligence , 1995 .

[11]  R. Nisbett,et al.  Causal attribution across domains and cultures. , 1995 .

[12]  Andrew W. Moore,et al.  Reinforcement Learning: A Survey , 1996, J. Artif. Intell. Res..

[13]  B. Bogin,et al.  Evolution of the human life cycle , 2020, American journal of human biology : the official journal of the Human Biology Council.

[14]  P. Cheng From covariation to causation: A causal power theory. , 1997 .

[15]  D. Buonomano,et al.  Cortical plasticity: from synapses to maps. , 1998, Annual review of neuroscience.

[16]  E. Charnov,et al.  Grandmothering, menopause, and the evolution of human life histories. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[17]  Kim Hill,et al.  LIFE HISTORY TRAITS IN HUMANS: Theory and Empirical Studies , 1999 .

[18]  T. German,et al.  Immunity to functional fixedness in young children , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[19]  David M. Sobel,et al.  A theory of causal learning in children: causal maps and Bayes nets. , 2004, Psychological review.

[20]  P. Kuhl Early language acquisition: cracking the speech code , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[21]  Y. Munakata,et al.  Developmental cognitive neuroscience: progress and potential , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[22]  A. Whiten,et al.  Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens) , 2005, Animal Cognition.

[23]  S. M. Carlson Developmentally Sensitive Measures of Executive Function in Preschool Children , 2005, Developmental neuropsychology.

[24]  Syunzo Kawamura,et al.  The process of sub-culture propagation among Japanese macaques , 1959, Primates.

[25]  F. Blanchard-Fields,et al.  Do Beliefs and Attributional Complexity Influence Age Differences in the Correspondence Bias , 2006 .

[26]  S. Blakemore,et al.  Development of the adolescent brain: implications for executive function and social cognition. , 2006 .

[27]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  I Know You Are But What Am I?!: Neural Bases of Self- and Social Knowledge Retrieval in Children and Adults , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[28]  B. Chapais Primeval Kinship: How Pair-Bonding Gave Birth to Human Society , 2008 .

[29]  Bernard Chapais: Primeval kinship. How pair-bonding gave birth to human society , 2008, Primates.

[30]  Carrie L. Masten,et al.  Neural correlates of direct and reflected self-appraisals in adolescents and adults: when social perspective-taking informs self-perception. , 2009, Child development.

[31]  P. Harvey,et al.  Brain size, development and metabolism in birds and mammals , 2009 .

[32]  M. Konner,et al.  The Evolution of Childhood: Relationships, Emotion, Mind , 2010 .

[33]  J. Tenenbaum,et al.  Probabilistic models of cognition: exploring representations and inductive biases , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[34]  V. Weisbecker,et al.  Brain size, life history, and metabolism at the marsupial/placental dichotomy , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[35]  J. Tenenbaum,et al.  A tutorial introduction to Bayesian models of cognitive development , 2011, Cognition.

[36]  Charles Kemp,et al.  How to Grow a Mind: Statistics, Structure, and Abstraction , 2011, Science.

[37]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Dynamic reconfiguration of human brain networks during learning , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[38]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Children’s imitation of causal action sequences is influenced by statistical and pedagogical evidence , 2011, Cognition.

[39]  Jackie Chappell,et al.  Making tools isn’t child’s play , 2011, Cognition.

[40]  Linda Wilbrecht,et al.  Juvenile mice show greater flexibility in multiple choice reversal learning than adults , 2011, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  C. Lebel,et al.  Longitudinal Development of Human Brain Wiring Continues from Childhood into Adulthood , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[42]  S. Hrdy,et al.  Mothers and Others , 2009 .

[43]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Own and Others' Prior Experiences Influence Children's Imitation of Causal Acts. , 2011, Cognitive development.

[44]  E. Crone,et al.  Understanding adolescence as a period of social–affective engagement and goal flexibility , 2012, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[45]  T. Kushnir,et al.  Rational constructivism in cognitive development , 2012 .

[46]  A. Gopnik Scientific Thinking in Young Children: Theoretical Advances, Empirical Research, and Policy Implications , 2012, Science.

[47]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Reconstructing constructivism: causal models, Bayesian learning mechanisms, and the theory theory. , 2012, Psychological bulletin.

[48]  M. Bikson,et al.  Noninvasive transcranial direct current stimulation over the left prefrontal cortex facilitates cognitive flexibility in tool use , 2013, Cognitive neuroscience.

[49]  S. Denison,et al.  Rational variability in children’s causal inferences: The Sampling Hypothesis , 2013, Cognition.

[50]  Todd A. Hare,et al.  Early developmental emergence of human amygdala–prefrontal connectivity after maternal deprivation , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[51]  Noah D. Goodman,et al.  Did she jump because she was the big sister or because the trampoline was safe? Causal inference and the development of social attribution. , 2013, Child development.

[52]  W. Frankenhuis,et al.  The evolution of predictive adaptive responses in human life history , 2013, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[53]  Fei Xu,et al.  Infants Are Rational Constructivist Learners , 2013 .

[54]  Sabine Peters,et al.  Gambling for self, friends, and antagonists: Differential contributions of affective and social brain regions on adolescent reward processing , 2014, NeuroImage.

[55]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[56]  Christopher G. Lucas,et al.  When children are better (or at least more open-minded) learners than adults: Developmental differences in learning the forms of causal relationships , 2014, Cognition.

[57]  S. Denison,et al.  Probabilistic models, learning algorithms, and response variability: sampling in cognitive development , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[58]  Sarah-Jayne Blakemore,et al.  Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing? , 2014, Annual review of psychology.

[59]  Thomas L. Griffiths,et al.  Win-Stay, Lose-Sample: A simple sequential algorithm for approximating Bayesian inference , 2014, Cognitive Psychology.

[60]  A. Thornton,et al.  Experimentally induced innovations lead to persistent culture via conformity in wild birds , 2014, Nature.

[61]  B. Hewlett,et al.  Cultural Variation in the Use of Overimitation by the Aka and Ngandu of the Congo Basin , 2015, PloS one.

[62]  C. Legare,et al.  Imitation and Innovation: The Dual Engines of Cultural Learning , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[63]  Christopher G. Lucas,et al.  When Younger Learners Can Be Better (or at Least More Open-Minded) Than Older Ones , 2015 .

[64]  K. Laland,et al.  Experimental Evidence for the Co-Evolution of Hominin Tool-Making Teaching and Language , 2014, Nature Communications.

[65]  Takao K Hensch,et al.  Critical periods in speech perception: new directions. , 2015, Annual review of psychology.

[66]  A. V. van Duijvenvoorde,et al.  Longitudinal Changes in Adolescent Risk-Taking: A Comprehensive Study of Neural Responses to Rewards, Pubertal Development, and Risk-Taking Behavior , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[67]  Brendan J. Barrett,et al.  Older, sociable capuchins (Cebus capucinus) invent more social behaviors, but younger monkeys innovate more in other contexts , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[68]  R. Dahl,et al.  Social status strategy in early adolescent girls: Testosterone and value-based decision making , 2017, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[69]  Kevin N Laland,et al.  Coevolution of cultural intelligence, extended life history, sociality, and brain size in primates , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[70]  Richard McElreath,et al.  Conformity does not perpetuate suboptimal traditions in a wild population of songbirds , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[71]  Daniel J. Plebanek,et al.  Costs of Selective Attention: When Children Notice What Adults Miss , 2017, Psychological science.

[72]  C. Legare Cumulative cultural learning: Development and diversity , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[73]  Josiah R. Boivin,et al.  Does puberty mark a transition in sensitive periods for plasticity in the associative neocortex? , 2017, Brain Research.

[74]  K. Dodge,et al.  Around the world, adolescence is a time of heightened sensation seeking and immature self-regulation. , 2018, Developmental science.