Why we may not find intentions in the brain

Intentions are commonly conceived of as discrete mental states that are the direct cause of actions. In the last several decades, neuroscientists have taken up the project of finding the neural implementation of intentions, and a number of areas have been posited as implementing these states. We argue, however, that the processes underlying action initiation and control are considerably more dynamic and context sensitive than the concept of intention can allow for. Therefore, adopting the notion of 'intention' in neuroscientific explanations can easily lead to misinterpretation of the data, and can negatively influence investigation into the neural correlates of intentional action. We suggest reinterpreting the mechanisms underlying intentional action, and we will discuss the elements that such a reinterpretation needs to account for.

[1]  M. Brass,et al.  Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[2]  J. Searle Intentionality: An Essay in the Philosophy of Mind , 1983 .

[3]  Patrick Haggard,et al.  Decision Time for Free Will , 2011, Neuron.

[4]  John D. Greenwood,et al.  The Future of Folk Psychology: Intentionality and Cognitive Science , 1991 .

[5]  Dominique Martinez,et al.  A Model of Stimulus-Specific Neural Assemblies in the Insect Antennal Lobe , 2008, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[6]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Evidence for a distributed hierarchy of action representation in the brain. , 2007, Human movement science.

[7]  P. Haggard Human volition: towards a neuroscience of will , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[8]  J. Haynes,et al.  Predicting free choices for abstract intentions , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[9]  Michael E. Bratman,et al.  Intention, Plans, and Practical Reason , 1991 .

[10]  T. Shallice,et al.  Hierarchical schemas and goals in the control of sequential behavior. , 2006, Psychological review.

[11]  Paul B. Johnson,et al.  Premotor and parietal cortex: corticocortical connectivity and combinatorial computations. , 1997, Annual review of neuroscience.

[12]  David Badre,et al.  Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Evidence for a Hierarchical Organization of the Prefrontal Cortex , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  An accumulator model for spontaneous neural activity prior to self-initiated movement , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[14]  Elisabeth Patherie,et al.  The Content of Intentions , 2000 .

[15]  A. Meltzoff Understanding the Intentions of Others: Re-Enactment of Intended Acts by 18-Month-Old Children. , 1995, Developmental psychology.

[16]  L. Nadel,et al.  Conscious Will and Responsibility. A tribute to Benjamin Libet , 2010 .

[17]  J. Price,et al.  The organization of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[18]  D. Davidson Actions, Reasons, And Causes , 1980 .

[19]  Afdc Hamilton,et al.  The motor hierarchy: from kinematics to goals and intentions , 2007 .

[20]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  The frontopolar cortex and human cognition: Evidence for a rostrocaudal hierarchical organization within the human prefrontal cortex , 2000, Psychobiology.

[21]  D. Plaut,et al.  Such stuff as habits are made on: A reply to Cooper and Shallice (2006). , 2006 .

[22]  J. Fuster,et al.  Cellular discharge in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the monkey in cognitive tasks , 1982, Experimental Neurology.

[23]  A. Owen,et al.  Anterior prefrontal cortex: insights into function from anatomy and neuroimaging , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[24]  R. Passingham,et al.  Reading Hidden Intentions in the Human Brain , 2007, Current Biology.

[25]  G. Pezzulo,et al.  Intentional action: from anticipation to goal-directed behavior , 2009, Psychological research.

[26]  B. Hommel Event files: feature binding in and across perception and action , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[27]  Marcia K. Johnson,et al.  Left Anterior Prefrontal Activation Increases with Demands to Recall Specific Perceptual Information , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  E. Pacherie The phenomenology of action: A conceptual framework , 2008, Cognition.

[29]  T. Shallice,et al.  Structured representations in the control of behavior cannot be so easily dismissed: a reply to Botvinick and Plaut (2006) , 2006 .

[30]  T. Shallice,et al.  The cognitive and neuroanatomical correlates of multitasking , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  M. Davies,et al.  Folk Psychology: The Theory of Mind Debate , 1995 .

[32]  E. Koechlin,et al.  The Architecture of Cognitive Control in the Human Prefrontal Cortex , 2003, Science.

[33]  M. Rushworth Intention, Choice, and the Medial Frontal Cortex , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[34]  P. Haggard Conscious intention and motor cognition , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[35]  Gustavo Deco,et al.  Cognitive flexibility and decision‐making in a model of conditional visuomotor associations , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[36]  S. Stich From folk psychology to cognitive science: The case against belief. , 1986 .

[37]  P. Haggard,et al.  Subliminal priming of actions influences sense of control over effects of action , 2010, Cognition.

[38]  Aldo Genovesio,et al.  Representation of Future and Previous Spatial Goals by Separate Neural Populations in Prefrontal Cortex , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  Peter Carruthers,et al.  The illusion of conscious will , 2007, Synthese.

[40]  Andrew R. Mitz,et al.  Prefrontal Cortex Activity Related to Abstract Response Strategies , 2005, Neuron.

[41]  D. Wegner,et al.  Modulating the sense of agency with external cues , 2009, Consciousness and Cognition.

[42]  K. Sakai Task set and prefrontal cortex. , 2008, Annual review of neuroscience.

[43]  S. Stich,et al.  Connectionism, Eliminativism, and the Future of Folk Psychology , 1990 .

[44]  John-Dylan Haynes,et al.  Human anterior prefrontal cortex encodes the ‘what’ and ‘when’ of future intentions , 2012, NeuroImage.

[45]  Arthur B. Markman,et al.  Discrete thoughts: Why cognition must use discrete representations , 2003 .

[46]  J. Fuster The Prefrontal Cortex—An Update Time Is of the Essence , 2001, Neuron.

[47]  Todd S. Braver,et al.  Motivation and Cognitive Control , 2015 .

[48]  S. Monsell,et al.  Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .

[49]  Claude Delpuech,et al.  Neural dynamics of the intention to speak. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[50]  L. Burton Intention , 2011 .

[51]  J. Fuster Upper processing stages of the perception–action cycle , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[52]  Michael J. Wiemers,et al.  Interplay Between Action and Movement Intentions During Social Interaction , 2012, Psychological science.

[53]  Paul Cisek,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of action selection: the affordance competition hypothesis , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[54]  David Badre,et al.  Cognitive control, hierarchy, and the rostro–caudal organization of the frontal lobes , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[55]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[56]  Patrick Haggard,et al.  What are intentions , 2010 .

[57]  J. Fodor Psychosemantics: The Problem of Meaning in the Philosophy of Mind , 1988 .

[58]  S. Courtney,et al.  Attention and cognitive control as emergent properties of information representation in working memory , 2004, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[59]  E. Koechlin,et al.  Motivation and cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[60]  Gustavo Deco,et al.  Neurodynamics of the Prefrontal Cortex during Conditional Visuomotor Associations , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[61]  D. Wegner The Illusion of Conscious Will , 2018, The MIT Press.

[62]  Willem F. G. Haselager,et al.  Cognitive Science and Folk Psychology: The Right Frame of Mind , 1997 .

[63]  Elisabeth Pacherie,et al.  Nonconceptual Representations for Action and the Limits of Intentional Control , 2011 .

[64]  John D E Gabrieli,et al.  Evaluating self-generated information: anterior prefrontal contributions to human cognition. , 2003, Behavioral neuroscience.

[65]  James K. Kroger,et al.  Cross-modal and cross-temporal association in neurons of frontal cortex , 2000, Nature.

[66]  P. Churchland,et al.  From Folk Psychology to Cognitive Science: The Case against Belief. , 1985 .

[67]  D. Plaut,et al.  Doing without schema hierarchies: a recurrent connectionist approach to normal and impaired routine sequential action. , 2004, Psychological review.

[68]  Iris van Rooij,et al.  Hierarchies in Action and Motor Control , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[69]  E. Koechlin,et al.  Reasoning, Learning, and Creativity: Frontal Lobe Function and Human Decision-Making , 2012, PLoS biology.

[70]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Neural mechanisms for interacting with a world full of action choices. , 2010, Annual review of neuroscience.

[71]  L. Shah,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 2010, Seminars in roentgenology.

[72]  R. Passingham,et al.  Specialisation within the prefrontal cortex: the ventral prefrontal cortex and associative learning , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[73]  P. Churchland A neurocomputational perspective , 1989 .

[74]  Roger G. Barker The stream of behavior : explorations of its structure & content , 1963 .

[75]  Sara Bizarro,et al.  Mental Mechanisms: Philosophical Perspectives on Cognitive Neuroscience , 2008 .

[76]  K. Christoff,et al.  Prefrontal organization of cognitive control according to levels of abstraction , 2009, Brain Research.

[77]  J. D. E. Gabrieli,et al.  Integration of diverse information in working memory within the frontal lobe , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[78]  R. Passingham,et al.  Attention to Intention , 2004, Science.

[79]  S. Uithol,et al.  What do infants understand of others’ action? A theoretical account of early social cognition , 2013, Psychological Research.

[80]  B. Libet Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of conscious will in voluntary action , 1985, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[81]  William Bechtel,et al.  Modules, Brain Parts, and Evolutionary Psychology , 2003 .

[82]  C. Summerfield,et al.  An information theoretical approach to prefrontal executive function , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[83]  Jun Tani,et al.  Emergence of Functional Hierarchy in a Multiple Timescale Neural Network Model: A Humanoid Robot Experiment , 2008, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[84]  M. Schölvinck,et al.  Differential components of prospective memory? Evidence from fMRI , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[85]  J. Tanji,et al.  Activity in the Lateral Prefrontal Cortex Reflects Multiple Steps of Future Events in Action Plans , 2006, Neuron.

[86]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A Hierarchy of Time-Scales and the Brain , 2008, PLoS Comput. Biol..