Neural knowledge assembly in humans and deep networks

Human understanding of the world can change rapidly when new information comes to light, such as when a plot twist occurs in a work of fiction. This flexible “knowledge assembly” requires few-shot reorganisation of neural codes for relations among objects and events. However, existing computational theories are largely silent about how this could occur. Here, participants learned a transitive ordering among novel objects within two distinct contexts, before exposure to new knowledge that revealed how they were linked. BOLD signals in dorsal frontoparietal cortical areas revealed that objects were rapidly and dramatically rearranged on the neural manifold after minimal exposure to linking information. We then adapt stochastic online gradient descent to permit similar rapid knowledge assembly in a neural network model.

[1]  Z. Kurth-Nelson,et al.  Impaired neural replay of inferred relationships in schizophrenia , 2021, Cell.

[2]  N. Daw,et al.  Formalizing planning and information search in naturalistic decision-making , 2021, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  Christian K. Machens,et al.  Representational geometry of perceptual decisions in the monkey parietal cortex , 2021, Cell.

[4]  Andrew M. Saxe,et al.  Rich and lazy learning of task representations in brains and neural networks , 2021, bioRxiv.

[5]  Alison R Preston,et al.  Concept formation as a computational cognitive process , 2021, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[6]  C. Summerfield,et al.  Neural state space alignment for magnitude generalization in humans and recurrent networks , 2021, Neuron.

[7]  Grace W. Lindsay Convolutional Neural Networks as a Model of the Visual System: Past, Present, and Future , 2020, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[8]  Andrew M. Saxe,et al.  If deep learning is the answer, what is the question? , 2020, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[9]  Silvia Bernardi,et al.  The Geometry of Abstraction in the Hippocampus and Prefrontal Cortex , 2020, Cell.

[10]  Linda Yu,et al.  Do grid codes afford generalization and flexible decision-making? , 2020, 2106.16219.

[11]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Episodic memory retrieval success is associated with rapid replay of episode content , 2020, Nature Neuroscience.

[12]  Mirko Klukas,et al.  Efficient and flexible representation of higher-dimensional cognitive variables with grid cells , 2020, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[13]  Christopher Summerfield,et al.  Structure learning and the posterior parietal cortex , 2020, Progress in Neurobiology.

[14]  Caswell Barry,et al.  The Tolman-Eichenbaum Machine: Unifying Space and Relational Memory through Generalization in the Hippocampal Formation , 2019, Cell.

[15]  Danielle S. Bassett,et al.  Graph learning: How humans infer and represent networks , 2019 .

[16]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Human Replay Spontaneously Reorganizes Experience , 2019, Cell.

[17]  Thomas L. Griffiths,et al.  Automatically Composing Representation Transformations as a Means for Generalization , 2018, ICLR.

[18]  Peter Gärdenfors,et al.  Navigating cognition: Spatial codes for human thinking , 2018, Science.

[19]  Zeb Kurth-Nelson,et al.  What Is a Cognitive Map? Organizing Knowledge for Flexible Behavior , 2018, Neuron.

[20]  C. Summerfield,et al.  Neural structure mapping in human probabilistic reward learning , 2018, bioRxiv.

[21]  Felix Hill,et al.  Measuring abstract reasoning in neural networks , 2018, ICML.

[22]  Alison R Preston,et al.  Memory integration constructs maps of space, time, and concepts , 2017, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[23]  Alison R. Preston,et al.  Temporal Proximity Promotes Integration of Overlapping Events , 2017, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[24]  Raymond J Dolan,et al.  A map of abstract relational knowledge in the human hippocampal–entorhinal cortex , 2017, eLife.

[25]  Razvan Pascanu,et al.  Overcoming catastrophic forgetting in neural networks , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[26]  Michael C Hout,et al.  The Novel Object and Unusual Name (NOUN) Database: A collection of novel images for use in experimental research , 2016, Behavior research methods.

[27]  Zeb Kurth-Nelson,et al.  Fast Sequences of Non-spatial State Representations in Humans , 2016, Neuron.

[28]  James L. McClelland,et al.  What Learning Systems do Intelligent Agents Need? Complementary Learning Systems Theory Updated , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[29]  Joshua B Tenenbaum,et al.  Toward the neural implementation of structure learning , 2016, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[30]  Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al.  Building machines that learn and think like people , 2016, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[31]  Ron Meir,et al.  Extracting grid cell characteristics from place cell inputs using non-negative principal component analysis , 2016, eLife.

[32]  Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al.  Human-level concept learning through probabilistic program induction , 2015, Science.

[33]  Robert C. Wilson,et al.  Reinforcement Learning in Multidimensional Environments Relies on Attention Mechanisms , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Neural representations of events arise from temporal community structure , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[35]  Lauren V. Kustner,et al.  Shaping of Object Representations in the Human Medial Temporal Lobe Based on Temporal Regularities , 2012, Current Biology.

[36]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Generalization Through the Recurrent Interaction of Episodic Memories , 2012, Psychological review.

[37]  N. Daw,et al.  Generalization of value in reinforcement learning by humans , 2012, The European journal of neuroscience.

[38]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[39]  Vincent Walsh A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[40]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[41]  H. Terrace,et al.  Knowledge of the Ordinal Position of List Items in Rhesus Monkeys , 1997 .

[42]  F. R. Treichler,et al.  Concurrent conditional discrimination tests of transitive inference by macaque monkeys: list linking. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[43]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. , 1995, Psychological review.

[44]  M. Colombo,et al.  The symbolic distance effect in monkeys (Cebus apella) , 1990 .

[45]  Keith J. Holyoak,et al.  Positional discriminability in linear orderings , 1978 .

[46]  E. Tolman Cognitive maps in rats and men. , 1948, Psychological review.