Neural knowledge assembly in humans and deep networks
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Z. Kurth-Nelson,et al. Impaired neural replay of inferred relationships in schizophrenia , 2021, Cell.
[2] N. Daw,et al. Formalizing planning and information search in naturalistic decision-making , 2021, Nature Neuroscience.
[3] Christian K. Machens,et al. Representational geometry of perceptual decisions in the monkey parietal cortex , 2021, Cell.
[4] Andrew M. Saxe,et al. Rich and lazy learning of task representations in brains and neural networks , 2021, bioRxiv.
[5] Alison R Preston,et al. Concept formation as a computational cognitive process , 2021, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.
[6] C. Summerfield,et al. Neural state space alignment for magnitude generalization in humans and recurrent networks , 2021, Neuron.
[7] Grace W. Lindsay. Convolutional Neural Networks as a Model of the Visual System: Past, Present, and Future , 2020, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[8] Andrew M. Saxe,et al. If deep learning is the answer, what is the question? , 2020, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[9] Silvia Bernardi,et al. The Geometry of Abstraction in the Hippocampus and Prefrontal Cortex , 2020, Cell.
[10] Linda Yu,et al. Do grid codes afford generalization and flexible decision-making? , 2020, 2106.16219.
[11] Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al. Episodic memory retrieval success is associated with rapid replay of episode content , 2020, Nature Neuroscience.
[12] Mirko Klukas,et al. Efficient and flexible representation of higher-dimensional cognitive variables with grid cells , 2020, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[13] Christopher Summerfield,et al. Structure learning and the posterior parietal cortex , 2020, Progress in Neurobiology.
[14] Caswell Barry,et al. The Tolman-Eichenbaum Machine: Unifying Space and Relational Memory through Generalization in the Hippocampal Formation , 2019, Cell.
[15] Danielle S. Bassett,et al. Graph learning: How humans infer and represent networks , 2019 .
[16] Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al. Human Replay Spontaneously Reorganizes Experience , 2019, Cell.
[17] Thomas L. Griffiths,et al. Automatically Composing Representation Transformations as a Means for Generalization , 2018, ICLR.
[18] Peter Gärdenfors,et al. Navigating cognition: Spatial codes for human thinking , 2018, Science.
[19] Zeb Kurth-Nelson,et al. What Is a Cognitive Map? Organizing Knowledge for Flexible Behavior , 2018, Neuron.
[20] C. Summerfield,et al. Neural structure mapping in human probabilistic reward learning , 2018, bioRxiv.
[21] Felix Hill,et al. Measuring abstract reasoning in neural networks , 2018, ICML.
[22] Alison R Preston,et al. Memory integration constructs maps of space, time, and concepts , 2017, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.
[23] Alison R. Preston,et al. Temporal Proximity Promotes Integration of Overlapping Events , 2017, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[24] Raymond J Dolan,et al. A map of abstract relational knowledge in the human hippocampal–entorhinal cortex , 2017, eLife.
[25] Razvan Pascanu,et al. Overcoming catastrophic forgetting in neural networks , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[26] Michael C Hout,et al. The Novel Object and Unusual Name (NOUN) Database: A collection of novel images for use in experimental research , 2016, Behavior research methods.
[27] Zeb Kurth-Nelson,et al. Fast Sequences of Non-spatial State Representations in Humans , 2016, Neuron.
[28] James L. McClelland,et al. What Learning Systems do Intelligent Agents Need? Complementary Learning Systems Theory Updated , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[29] Joshua B Tenenbaum,et al. Toward the neural implementation of structure learning , 2016, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[30] Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al. Building machines that learn and think like people , 2016, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[31] Ron Meir,et al. Extracting grid cell characteristics from place cell inputs using non-negative principal component analysis , 2016, eLife.
[32] Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al. Human-level concept learning through probabilistic program induction , 2015, Science.
[33] Robert C. Wilson,et al. Reinforcement Learning in Multidimensional Environments Relies on Attention Mechanisms , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[34] M. Botvinick,et al. Neural representations of events arise from temporal community structure , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.
[35] Lauren V. Kustner,et al. Shaping of Object Representations in the Human Medial Temporal Lobe Based on Temporal Regularities , 2012, Current Biology.
[36] James L. McClelland,et al. Generalization Through the Recurrent Interaction of Episodic Memories , 2012, Psychological review.
[37] N. Daw,et al. Generalization of value in reinforcement learning by humans , 2012, The European journal of neuroscience.
[38] S. Dehaene,et al. Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[39] Vincent Walsh. A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[40] M. Botvinick,et al. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.
[41] H. Terrace,et al. Knowledge of the Ordinal Position of List Items in Rhesus Monkeys , 1997 .
[42] F. R. Treichler,et al. Concurrent conditional discrimination tests of transitive inference by macaque monkeys: list linking. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[43] James L. McClelland,et al. Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. , 1995, Psychological review.
[44] M. Colombo,et al. The symbolic distance effect in monkeys (Cebus apella) , 1990 .
[45] Keith J. Holyoak,et al. Positional discriminability in linear orderings , 1978 .
[46] E. Tolman. Cognitive maps in rats and men. , 1948, Psychological review.