Infants’ sensitivity to statistical distributions of motion direction and speed

Adults combine different local motions to form a global percept of motion. This study explores the origins of this process by testing how perturbations of local motion influence infants' sensitivity to global motion. Infants at 6-, 12-, and 18-weeks of age viewed random dots moving with a gaussian distribution of dot directions defined by a mean of 0 degree (rightward) or 180 degrees (leftward) and a standard deviation (SD) of 0, 34, or 68 degrees. A well-practiced observer used infants' optokinetic responses to judge the direction of stimulus motion. Infants were studied both cross-sectionally and longitudinally. Direction discrimination was relatively high at all ages when the SD was 0 degree. When the SD was 34 or 68 degrees, performance declined with age. Adult performance was nearly perfect at these SDs. A similar developmental pattern was found with distributions of dot speed. The decline in infant performance is consistent with the development of both neural tuning and receptive field size. The subsequent improvement by adulthood suggests the development of additional processes such as long-range interactions.

[1]  K. H. Britten,et al.  Responses of neurons in macaque MT to stochastic motion signals , 1993, Visual Neuroscience.

[2]  Charles G. Gross,et al.  Pattern recognition mechanisms , 1985 .

[3]  Nikos K. Logothetis,et al.  Physiological Studies of Motion Inputs. , 1994 .

[4]  R. Manny,et al.  Motion coherence in infants , 1990, Vision Research.

[5]  R Blake,et al.  Discrimination of coherent motion when local motion varies in speed and direction. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  A. Burkhalter,et al.  Development of local circuits in human visual cortex , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[7]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[8]  P. Yakovlev,et al.  The myelogenetic cycles of regional maturation of the brain , 1967 .

[9]  J Lorenceau,et al.  Motion Integration with Dot Patterns: Effects of Motion Noise and Structural Information , 1996, Vision Research.

[10]  J. L. Conel,et al.  The postnatal development of the human cerebral cortex , 1960 .

[11]  M. Banks,et al.  The development of light adaptation in human infants , 1983, Vision Research.

[12]  Infants’ perception of three-dimensional shape specified by motion-carried information , 1992 .

[13]  K. H. Britten,et al.  Neuronal correlates of a perceptual decision , 1989, Nature.

[14]  B. Bertenthal,et al.  Infants' sensitivity to uniform motion , 1996, Vision Research.

[15]  John Wattam-Bell,et al.  Coherence thresholds for discrimination of motion direction in infants , 1994, Vision Research.

[16]  J. Atkinson,et al.  Development of Orientation Discrimination in Infancy , 1988, Perception.

[17]  K. Albus,et al.  Early post‐natal development of neuronal function in the kitten's visual cortex: a laminar analysis. , 1984, The Journal of physiology.

[18]  A. Minkowski,et al.  Regional Development of the Brain in Early Life , 1968 .

[19]  Scott N. J. Watamaniuk,et al.  Temporal and spatial integration in dynamic random-dot stimuli , 1992, Vision Research.

[20]  J. Atkinson,et al.  A Photorefractive study of infant accommodation , 1979, Vision Research.

[21]  H. Ghim,et al.  Evidence for perceptual organization in infants: Perception of subjective contours by young infants , 1990 .

[22]  Robert Sekuler,et al.  Coherent global motion percepts from stochastic local motions , 1984, Vision Research.

[23]  Angela M. Brown Intrinsic contrast noise and infant visual contrast discrimination , 1994, Vision Research.

[24]  Janette Atkinson,et al.  Development of contrast sensitivity over the first 3 months of life in the human infant , 1977, Vision Research.

[25]  Albert Yonas,et al.  Perception of structure from motion , 1995 .

[26]  P. Huttenlocher,et al.  The development of synapses in striate cortex of man. , 1987, Human neurobiology.

[27]  Maggie Shiffrar,et al.  The influence of terminators on motion integration across space , 1992, Vision Research.

[28]  P. Heggelund,et al.  Development of spatial receptive-field organization and orientation selectivity in kitten striate cortex. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  G. Orban,et al.  Human velocity and direction discrimination measured with random dot patterns , 1988, Vision Research.

[30]  A. Norcia,et al.  Spatial frequency sweep VEP: Visual acuity during the first year of life , 1985, Vision Research.

[31]  Andrew P. Duchon,et al.  The human visual system averages speed information , 1992, Vision Research.

[32]  J. Atkinson,et al.  Orientation-specific cortical responses develop in early infancy , 1986, Nature.

[33]  Y. Chino,et al.  Nasotemporal directional bias of V1 neurons in young infant monkeys. , 1998, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[34]  B. Bertenthal,et al.  Development of visual organization: the perception of subjective contours. , 1980, Child development.

[35]  E. Adelson,et al.  Phenomenal coherence of moving visual patterns , 1982, Nature.

[36]  E. Adelson,et al.  The analysis of moving visual patterns , 1985 .

[37]  Andrew T. Smith,et al.  Visual detection of motion , 1994 .

[38]  A. B. Bonds Development of Orientation Tuning in the Visual Cortex of Kittens , 1979 .

[39]  J Wattam-Bell,et al.  Visual motion processing in one-month-old infants: Preferential looking experiments , 1996, Vision Research.

[40]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Hysteresis in the perception of motion direction as evidence for neural cooperativity , 1986, Nature.

[41]  J Atkinson,et al.  Contrast sensitivity function of preschool children. , 1981, The British journal of ophthalmology.

[42]  A. Norcia,et al.  Anomalous motion VEPs in infants and in infantile esotropia. , 1991, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[43]  J. Pettigrew,et al.  The effect of visual experience on the development of stimulus specificity by kitten cortical neurones , 1974, The Journal of physiology.

[44]  A Yonas,et al.  Infants’ sensitivity to kinetic information for three-dimensional object shape , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[45]  Hugh R. Wilson,et al.  Development of spatiotemporal mechanisms in infant vision , 1988, Vision Research.

[46]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Innate and environmental factors in the development of the kitten's visual cortex. , 1975, The Journal of physiology.

[47]  Elena K. Festa,et al.  Recruitment mechanisms in speed and fine-direction discrimination tasks , 1997, Vision Research.

[48]  G. Phillips,et al.  Cooperative phenomena in the perception of motion direction. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[49]  O. Braddick,et al.  The combination of motion signals over time , 1989, Vision Research.

[50]  A. Slater,et al.  Orientation Discrimination and Cortical Function in the Human Newborn , 1988, Perception.

[51]  D. Hubel,et al.  RECEPTIVE FIELDS OF CELLS IN STRIATE CORTEX OF VERY YOUNG, VISUALLY INEXPERIENCED KITTENS. , 1963, Journal of neurophysiology.

[52]  B I Bertenthal,et al.  Multiple Developmental Pathways for Motion Processing , 1997, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[53]  H. Wilson,et al.  A psychophysically motivated model for two-dimensional motion perception , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.

[54]  M. Schneck,et al.  Area-threshold relations at controlled retinal locations in 1-month-old infants , 1984, Vision Research.

[55]  Andrew T. Smith,et al.  Is global motion really based on spatial integration of local motion signals? , 1994, Vision Research.

[56]  Scott N. J. Watamaniuk,et al.  Direction Perception in Complex Dynamic Displays: the Integration of Dir~~tion Information , 1988 .

[57]  R. Freeman,et al.  Developmental Neurobiology of Vision , 1979, NATO Advanced Study Institutes Series.