The structure of integral dimensions: contrasting topological and Cartesian representations.

Diverse evidence shows that perceptually integral dimensions, such as those composing color, are represented holistically. However, the nature of these holistic representations is poorly understood. Extant theories, such as those founded on multidimensional scaling or general recognition theory, model integral stimulus spaces using a Cartesian coordinate system, just as with spaces defined by separable dimensions. This approach entails a rich geometrical structure that has never been questioned but may not be psychologically meaningful for integral dimensions. In particular, Cartesian models carry a notion of orthogonality of component dimensions, such that if 1 dimension is diagnostic for a classification or discrimination task, another can be selected as uniquely irrelevant. This article advances an alternative model in which integral dimensions are characterized as topological spaces. The Cartesian and topological models are tested in a series of experiments using the perceptual-learning phenomenon of dimension differentiation, whereby discrimination training with integral-dimension stimuli can induce an analytic representation of those stimuli. Under the present task design, the 2 models make contrasting predictions regarding the analytic representation that will be learned. Results consistently support the Cartesian model. These findings indicate that perceptual representations of integral dimensions are surprisingly structured, despite their holistic, unanalyzed nature.

[1]  Patricia M. Berretty,et al.  On the dominance of unidimensional rules in unsupervised categorization , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[2]  P. Hancock,et al.  Robust representations for face recognition: The power of averages , 2005, Cognitive Psychology.

[3]  W. R. Garner The Processing of Information and Structure , 1974 .

[4]  R. Nosofsky Similarity Scaling and Cognitive Process Models , 1992 .

[5]  R. Nosofsky Attention, similarity, and the identification-categorization relationship. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[6]  D. Scott Perceptual learning. , 1974, Queen's nursing journal.

[7]  G. Lockhead Processing dimensional stimuli: a note. , 1972, Psychological review.

[8]  Robert L. Goldstone,et al.  The effect of the internal structure of categories on perception , 2008 .

[9]  John R. Anderson,et al.  The Adaptive Nature of Human Categorization , 1991 .

[10]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The fusiform face area subserves face perception, not generic within-category identification , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[11]  R. Kimchi,et al.  Perceptual organization in vision : behavioral and neural perspectives , 2003 .

[12]  J. A. López del Val,et al.  Principal Components Analysis , 2018, Applied Univariate, Bivariate, and Multivariate Statistics Using Python.

[13]  L. Tucker Relations between multidimensional scaling and three-mode factor analysis , 1972 .

[14]  Richard Durbin,et al.  The computing neuron , 1989 .

[15]  W. R. Garner,et al.  Integrality of stimulus dimensions in various types of information processing , 1970 .

[16]  R. Goldstone An efficient method for obtaining similarity data , 1994 .

[17]  R. Nosofsky,et al.  Predicting similarity and categorization from identification. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[18]  Robert L. Goldstone Influences of categorization on perceptual discrimination. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[19]  R. Hyman,et al.  Judgments of similarity and spatial models , 1967 .

[20]  Daniel R. Miller,et al.  Final report , 2000 .

[21]  L. Marks,et al.  Optional processes in similarity judgments , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[22]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  Natural image statistics and neural representation. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[23]  L E Marks,et al.  Perceptual primacy of dimensions: support for a model of dimensional interaction. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  David H. Brainard,et al.  Calibration of a computer controlled color monitor , 1989 .

[25]  Bob McMurray,et al.  Cue Integration With Categories: Weighting Acoustic Cues in Speech Using Unsupervised Learning and Distributional Statistics , 2010, Cogn. Sci..

[26]  Johan Wagemans,et al.  The effect of category learning on the representation of shape: dimensions can be biased but not differentiated. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[27]  Linda R. Tennison,et al.  Range effects and dimensional organization in visual discrimination , 1998, Behavioural Processes.

[28]  Adam N. Sanborn,et al.  Unifying rational models of categorization via the hierarchical Dirichlet process , 2019 .

[29]  Dorothy Nickerson,et al.  The Specification of Color Tolerances , 1936 .

[30]  Ray Hyman,et al.  Perceptual separability and spatial models , 1968 .

[31]  Xiaojin Zhu,et al.  Can semi-supervised learning explain incorrect beliefs about categories? , 2011, Cognition.

[32]  Xiaojin Zhu,et al.  Humans Perform Semi-Supervised Classification Too , 2007, AAAI.

[33]  W. T. Maddox,et al.  A response time theory of separability and integrality in speeded classification , 1994 .

[34]  Joseph L. Zinnes,et al.  Theory and Methods of Scaling. , 1958 .

[35]  Peter Földiák,et al.  Adaptation and decorrelation in the cortex , 1989 .

[36]  Lawrence E. Marks,et al.  Early-holistic processing or dimensional similarity? , 1993 .

[37]  Michael G. Strintzis,et al.  Face Recognition , 2008, Encyclopedia of Multimedia.

[38]  Robert L. Goldstone,et al.  The sensitization and differentiation of dimensions during category learning. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[39]  C. J. McGrath,et al.  Effect of exchange rate return on volatility spill-over across trading regions , 2012 .

[40]  W. Torgerson A THEORETICAL AND EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION OF MULTIDIMENSIONAL SCALING , 1951 .

[41]  N. Perrin,et al.  Varieties of perceptual independence. , 1986, Psychological review.

[42]  T. Landauer,et al.  A Solution to Plato's Problem: The Latent Semantic Analysis Theory of Acquisition, Induction, and Representation of Knowledge. , 1997 .

[43]  J W Grau,et al.  The distinction between integral and separable dimensions: evidence for the integrality of pitch and loudness. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[44]  D. K. Nelson,et al.  Processing integral dimensions: the whole view. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[45]  Thomas L. Griffiths,et al.  Modeling Transfer Learning in Human Categorization with the Hierarchical Dirichlet Process , 2010, ICML.

[46]  J. D. Smith,et al.  Prototypes in the Mist: The Early Epochs of Category Learning , 1998 .

[47]  M. Turk,et al.  Eigenfaces for Recognition , 1991, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[48]  J. Kruskal Multidimensional scaling by optimizing goodness of fit to a nonmetric hypothesis , 1964 .

[49]  H. Abdi,et al.  What Represents a Face? A Computational Approach for the Integration of Physiological and Psychological Data , 1997, Perception.

[50]  Alice J. O'Toole,et al.  Face recognition algorithms and the other-race effect: computational mechanisms for a developmental contact hypothesis , 2002, Cogn. Sci..

[51]  N. Mackintosh,et al.  Mechanisms of animal discrimination learning , 1971 .

[52]  W. T. Maddox,et al.  Stimulus Generalization in Category Learning , 2005 .

[53]  Robert L. Goldstone,et al.  The development of features in object concepts , 1998, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[54]  R. Shepard The analysis of proximities: Multidimensional scaling with an unknown distance function. I. , 1962 .

[55]  R. Shepard Attention and the metric structure of the stimulus space. , 1964 .

[56]  David Travis,et al.  Effective Color Displays: Theory and Practice , 1991 .

[57]  C. F. Foard,et al.  Holistic and analytic modes of processing: the multiple determinants of perceptual analysis. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[58]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Levels of experienced dimensionality in children and adults , 1978, Cognitive Psychology.

[59]  W. T. Maddox,et al.  Relations between prototype, exemplar, and decision bound models of categorization , 1993 .

[60]  Cindy M. Bukach,et al.  Beyond faces and modularity: the power of an expertise framework , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[61]  Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.  The “independent components” of natural scenes are edge filters , 1997, Vision Research.

[62]  David L. Faigman,et al.  Human category learning. , 2005, Annual review of psychology.

[63]  F ATTNEAVE,et al.  Dimensions of similarity. , 1950, The American journal of psychology.

[64]  D. Medin,et al.  SUSTAIN: a network model of category learning. , 2004, Psychological review.

[65]  Emmanuel M. Pothos,et al.  One or two dimensions in spontaneous classification: A simplicity approach , 2008, Cognition.

[66]  S. Imai,et al.  The free classification of analyzable and unanalyzable stimuli , 1972 .

[67]  R. Shepard The analysis of proximities: Multidimensional scaling with an unknown distance function. II , 1962 .

[68]  Robert L. Goldstone Learning to Perceive While Perceiving to Learn , 2003 .

[69]  Robert L. Goldstone,et al.  Categorization and the Parsing of Objects , 2019, Proceedings of the Sixteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.

[70]  Revaz Valerianovich Gamkrelidze,et al.  Topology and Geometry , 1970 .

[71]  L E Marks,et al.  Primacy of dimensions in color perception. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[72]  D. B. Judd,et al.  Final Report of the O.S.A. Subcommittee on the Spacing of the Munsell Colors , 1943 .

[73]  Thomas L. Griffiths,et al.  Learning hypothesis spaces and dimensions through concept learning , 2010 .