Accurate judgments of intention from motion cues alone: A cross-cultural study
暂无分享,去创建一个
P. Todd | G. Miller | H. Barrett | P. W. Blythe | H. Clark Barrett | Peter M. Todd | Geoffrey F. Miller
[1] H. Barrett,et al. Functional Fixedness in a Technologically Sparse Culture , 2005, Psychological science.
[2] A. Meltzoff,et al. The detection of contingency and animacy from simple animations in the human brain. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.
[3] R. Cressman. Evolutionary Dynamics and Extensive Form Games , 2003 .
[4] John E. Opfer,et al. Identifying living and sentient kinds from dynamic information: the case of goal-directed versus aimless autonomous movement in conceptual change , 2002, Cognition.
[5] Kazuhiro Goto,et al. Discrimination of intentional and random motion paths by pigeons , 2002, Animal Cognition.
[6] C. Frith,et al. Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[7] H. Wellman,et al. Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. , 2001, Child development.
[8] D. Rakison,et al. Developmental origin of the animate-inanimate distinction. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.
[9] E. Dockner,et al. Differential Games in Economics and Management Science , 2001 .
[10] C. Frith,et al. Movement and Mind: A Functional Imaging Study of Perception and Interpretation of Complex Intentional Movement Patterns , 2000, NeuroImage.
[11] C. Uller,et al. Goal attribution in chimpanzees , 2000, Cognition.
[12] Patrice D. Tremoulet,et al. Perceptual causality and animacy , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[13] Patrice D. Tremoulet,et al. Perception of Animacy from the Motion of a Single Object , 2000, Perception.
[14] Susan C. Johnson. The recognition of mentalistic agents in infancy , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[15] R. Adolphs. Social cognition and the human brain , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[16] G. Csibra,et al. Goal attribution without agency cues: the perception of ‘pure reason’ in infancy , 1999, Cognition.
[17] Stewart W. Wilson,et al. From Animals to Animats 5. Proceedings of the Fifth International Conference on Simulation of Adaptive Behavior , 1997 .
[18] A. Wiley. Why We Get Sick: The New Science of Darwinian Medicine , 1997 .
[19] S. Lea,et al. Perception of Emotion from Dynamic Point-Light Displays Represented in Dance , 1996, Perception.
[20] Z. Nadasdy,et al. Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age , 1995, Cognition.
[21] F. Eeckman,et al. Evolution and Biocomputation: Computational Models of Evolution , 1995 .
[22] M. Morris,et al. Culture and Cause: American and Chinese Attributions for Social and Physical Events , 1994 .
[23] S. Lea,et al. Visual Perception of Intentional Motion , 1994, Perception.
[24] Ken Springer,et al. Structure, Motion, and Preschoolers' Perceptions of Social Causality , 1993 .
[25] V. Crawford. An “evolutionary” interpretation of Van Huyck, Battalio, and Beil's experimental results on coordination , 1991 .
[26] R. Fagen. Animal Play Behavior , 1981 .
[27] G. Johansson. Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .
[28] J. Jay Braun,et al. Evolution and Human Behavior , 1967, The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine.
[29] D Wishart,et al. Differential Games. A Mathematical Theory with Applications to Warfare and Pursuit, Control and Optimization , 1966 .
[30] F. Heider,et al. An experimental study of apparent behavior , 1944 .
[31] György Gergely,et al. One-year-old infants use teleological representations of actions productively , 2003, Cogn. Sci..
[32] U. Goswami. Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development , 2002 .
[33] Francis F. Steen,et al. Evolution's Pedagogy: An Adaptationist Model of Pretense and Entertainment , 2001 .
[34] Angelo Antoci,et al. Evolutionary Selection of Correlation Mechanisms for Coordination Games , 2000 .
[35] M. Haselton,et al. Error management theory: a new perspective on biases in cross-sex mind reading. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[36] U. Frith,et al. Do triangles play tricks? Attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and abnormal development , 2000 .
[37] Mauro Gallegati,et al. Interaction and Market Structure , 2000 .
[38] P. Todd,et al. How motion reveals intention: Categorizing social interactions , 1999 .
[39] P. Todd,et al. Simple Heuristics That Make Us Smart , 1999 .
[40] G. Miller. Protean primates: The evolution of adaptive unpredictability in competition and courtship , 1997 .
[41] R. Byrne,et al. Machiavellian intelligence II : extensions and evaluations , 1997 .
[42] Pattie Maes,et al. Co-evolution of Pursuit and Evasion II: Simulation Methods and Results , 1996 .
[43] Dave Cliff,et al. Co-evolution of pursuit and evasion II: Simulation Methods and results , 1996 .
[44] Peter M. Todd,et al. The Role of Mate Choice in Biocomputation: Sexual Selection as a Process of Search, Optimization and Diversification , 1995, Evolution and Biocomputation.
[45] Ralph L. Rosnow,et al. Essentials of Behavioral Research: Methods and Data Analysis , 1984 .
[46] P. Mineau,et al. Forced Copulation in Waterfowl , 1983 .