Why Some People Discount More than Others: Baseline Activation in the Dorsal PFC Mediates the Link between COMT Genotype and Impatient Choice

Individuals differ widely in how steeply they discount future rewards. The sources of these stable individual differences in delay discounting (DD) are largely unknown. One candidate is the COMT Val158Met polymorphism, known to modulate prefrontal dopamine levels and affect DD. To identify possible neural mechanisms by which this polymorphism may contribute to stable individual DD differences, we measured 73 participants’ neural baseline activation using resting electroencephalogram (EEG). Such neural baseline activation measures are highly heritable and stable over time, thus an ideal endophenotype candidate to explain how genes may influence behavior via individual differences in neural function. After EEG-recording, participants made a series of incentive-compatible intertemporal choices to determine the steepness of their DD. We found that COMT significantly affected DD and that this effect was mediated by baseline activation level in the left dorsal prefrontal cortex (DPFC): (i) COMT had a significant effect on DD such that the number of Val alleles was positively correlated with steeper DD (higher numbers of Val alleles means greater COMT activity and thus lower dopamine levels). (ii) A whole-brain search identified a cluster in left DPFC where baseline activation was correlated with DD; lower activation was associated with steeper DD. (iii) COMT had a significant effect on the baseline activation level in this left DPFC cluster such that a higher number of Val alleles was associated with lower baseline activation. (iv) The effect of COMT on DD was explained by the mediating effect of neural baseline activation in the left DPFC cluster. Our study thus establishes baseline activation level in left DPFC as salient neural signature in the form of an endophenotype that mediates the link between COMT and DD.

[1]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system , 2009, NeuroImage.

[2]  M. Bonnet,et al.  Impact of activity and arousal upon spectral EEG parameters , 2001, Physiology & Behavior.

[3]  J. E. Mazur An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. , 1987 .

[4]  A. Kleinschmidt,et al.  Electroencephalographic signatures of attentional and cognitive default modes in spontaneous brain activity fluctuations at rest , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[5]  W. Bickel,et al.  Behavioral and neuroeconomics of drug addiction: competing neural systems and temporal discounting processes. , 2007, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[6]  E. D. Geus From genotype to EEG endophenotype: a route for post-genomic understanding of complex psychiatric disease? , 2010, Genome Medicine.

[7]  R D Pascual-Marqui,et al.  Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA): technical details. , 2002, Methods and findings in experimental and clinical pharmacology.

[8]  Paul J. Harrison,et al.  Catechol-O-Methyltransferase Inhibition Improves Set-Shifting Performance and Elevates Stimulated Dopamine Release in the Rat Prefrontal Cortex , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  Kristopher J Preacher,et al.  Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models , 2008, Behavior research methods.

[10]  Nicole L. Wilson,et al.  'Willpower' over the life span: decomposing self-regulation. , 2011, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[11]  Noah A. Shamosh,et al.  Individual Differences in Delay Discounting , 2008, Psychological science.

[12]  L. Green,et al.  A discounting framework for choice with delayed and probabilistic rewards. , 2004, Psychological bulletin.

[13]  F. Vollenweider,et al.  Comparison of simultaneously recorded [H215O]‐PET and LORETA during cognitive and pharmacological activation , 2004, Human brain mapping.

[14]  Eric J. Johnson,et al.  Lateral prefrontal cortex and self-control in intertemporal choice , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  Brent L. Hughes,et al.  Prefrontal-Subcortical Pathways Mediating Successful Emotion Regulation , 2008, Neuron.

[16]  H. Stassen,et al.  Intraindividual Specificity and Stability of Human EEG: Comparing a Linear vs a Nonlinear Approach , 2000, Methods of Information in Medicine.

[17]  T. Braver,et al.  Vive les differences! Individual variation in neural mechanisms of executive control , 2010, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[18]  Jeffrey B. Henriques,et al.  Frontal Brain Asymmetry and Reward Responsiveness , 2005, Psychological science.

[19]  Jennifer M. Mitchell,et al.  Immediate Reward Bias in Humans: Fronto-Parietal Networks and a Role for the Catechol-O-Methyltransferase 158Val/Val Genotype , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  Nicholas G Martin,et al.  Common and specific genetic influences on EEG power bands delta, theta, alpha, and beta , 2007, Biological Psychology.

[21]  A. Scheres,et al.  Temporal Reward Discounting in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: The Contribution of Symptom Domains, Reward Magnitude, and Session Length , 2010, Biological Psychiatry.

[22]  Mitul A Mehta,et al.  DAT1 and COMT Effects on Delay Discounting and Trait Impulsivity in Male Adolescents with Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Healthy Controls , 2010, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[23]  W M Herrmann,et al.  Reflections on the topics: EEG frequency bands and regulation of vigilance. , 1979, Pharmakopsychiatrie, Neuro-Psychopharmakologie.

[24]  Jan Peters,et al.  Episodic Future Thinking Reduces Reward Delay Discounting through an Enhancement of Prefrontal-Mediotemporal Interactions , 2010, Neuron.

[25]  M. Egan,et al.  Functional analysis of genetic variation in catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT): effects on mRNA, protein, and enzyme activity in postmortem human brain. , 2004, American journal of human genetics.

[26]  A. Raftery Bayesian Model Selection in Social Research , 1995 .

[27]  Christine L Larson,et al.  Functional coupling of simultaneous electrical and metabolic activity in the human brain , 2004, Human brain mapping.

[28]  D. Hommer,et al.  Delay Discounting Correlates with Proportional Lateral Frontal Cortex Volumes , 2009, Biological Psychiatry.

[29]  J. Bouyer,et al.  Anatomical localization of cortical beta rhythms in cat , 1987, Neuroscience.

[30]  Kelvin O. Lim,et al.  White Matter Integrity Predicts Delay Discounting Behavior in 9- to 23-Year-Olds: A Diffusion Tensor Imaging Study , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[31]  D. Watson,et al.  Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[32]  E. Fetz,et al.  Coherent 25- to 35-Hz oscillations in the sensorimotor cortex of awake behaving monkeys. , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[33]  K. Kirby One-year temporal stability of delay-discount rates , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[34]  Lorena R. R. Gianotti,et al.  Dopamine Receptor D4 Polymorphism Predicts the Effect of L-DOPA on Gambling Behavior , 2010, Biological Psychiatry.

[35]  Martha J. Farah,et al.  Socioeconomic status and the brain: mechanistic insights from human and animal research , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[36]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  Separate Neural Systems Value Immediate and Delayed Monetary Rewards , 2004, Science.

[37]  B. Liu,et al.  Prefrontal-Related Functional Connectivities within the Default Network Are Modulated by COMT val158met in Healthy Young Adults , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  Marc Wildi,et al.  Test–retest reliability of resting EEG spectra validates a statistical signature of persons , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[39]  A. Odum Delay discounting: Trait variable? , 2011, Behavioural Processes.

[40]  Lorena R. R. Gianotti,et al.  A Neural Marker of Costly Punishment Behavior , 2010, Psychological science.

[41]  Jan Peters,et al.  The neural mechanisms of inter-temporal decision-making: understanding variability , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[42]  T. Paus,et al.  Synchronization of neuronal activity in the human primary motor cortex by transcranial magnetic stimulation: an EEG study. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  M. Munafo,et al.  Delayed reward discounting and addictive behavior: a meta-analysis , 2011, Psychopharmacology.

[44]  B. Reynolds A review of delay-discounting research with humans: relations to drug use and gambling , 2006, Behavioural pharmacology.

[45]  T. Robbins,et al.  Behavioral models of impulsivity in relation to ADHD: Translation between clinical and preclinical studies , 2006, Clinical psychology review.

[46]  Scott A. Huettel,et al.  Functional Neuroimaging of Intertemporal Choice Models: A Review , 2010 .

[47]  D I Boomsma,et al.  Heritability of background EEG across the power spectrum. , 2005, Psychophysiology.

[48]  A. Heath,et al.  Heritability of Delay Discounting in Adolescence: A Longitudinal Twin Study , 2011, Behavior genetics.

[50]  J. Patton,et al.  Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. , 1995, Journal of clinical psychology.

[51]  J. Monterosso,et al.  Beyond discounting: possible experimental models of impulse control , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[52]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: A primer with examples , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[53]  Nicholas T. Franklin,et al.  Behavioral and neural correlates of delay of gratification 40 years later , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[54]  Dietrich Lehmann,et al.  Tonic Activity Level in the Right Prefrontal Cortex Predicts Individuals' Risk Taking , 2009, Psychological science.

[55]  N. Chater,et al.  Associations between a one-shot delay discounting measure and age, income, education and real-world impulsive behavior , 2009 .

[56]  Matthew S. Fritz,et al.  Mediation analysis. , 2019, Annual review of psychology.

[57]  M. F. Triplett,et al.  Test-retest reliability of a group-administered paper-pencil measure of delay discounting. , 2009, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[58]  A. Meyer-Lindenberg,et al.  Intermediate phenotypes and genetic mechanisms of psychiatric disorders , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.