Common mechanisms for 2D tilt and 3D slant after-effects

By presenting oriented Gabor patches either monocularly or binocularly, we dissociated retinal orientation from perceived tilt and perceived slant. After adapting to binocular patches, with zero apparent tilt and non-zero slant, small tilt after-effects (TAEs) and large slant after-effects (SAE) were measured. Adapting to monocular patches with non-zero tilt and zero slant produced large TAEs and smaller SAEs. This pattern of results suggests that a common, low-level adaptation to monocular orientation is involved in slant and tilt after-effects. However, the incomplete transfer between slant and tilt makes it clear that higher-level adaptation is also involved, perhaps at the level of surface representation.

[1]  Wendy J. Adams,et al.  Adaptation to three-dimensional distortions in human vision , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[2]  L. Poom,et al.  Perceptual depth synthesis in the visual system as revealed by selective adaptation. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  B Moulden,et al.  After-effects and the integration of patterns of neural activity within a channel. , 1980, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[4]  F A Wichmann,et al.  Ning for Helpful Comments and Suggestions. This Paper Benefited Con- Siderably from Conscientious Peer Review, and We Thank Our Reviewers the Psychometric Function: I. Fitting, Sampling, and Goodness of Fit , 2001 .

[5]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[6]  G. Baumgartner,et al.  Disparity sensitivity and receptive field incongruity of units in the cat striate cortex , 1978, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  Fulvio Domini,et al.  3D after-effects are due to shape and not disparity adaptation , 2001, Vision Research.

[8]  F. Campbell,et al.  The tilt after-effect: a fresh look. , 1971, Vision research.

[9]  B G Cumming,et al.  Responses of Macaque V1 Neurons to Binocular Orientation Differences , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  F. A. Miles Binocular Vision and Stereopsis by Ian P. Howard and Brian J. Rogers, Oxford University Press, 1995. £90.00 (736 pages) ISBN 0 19 508476 4. , 1996, Trends in Neurosciences.

[11]  J. Gibson,et al.  ADAPTATION , AFTEREFFECT AND CONTRAST IN THE PERCEPTION OF TILTED LINES , 2004 .

[12]  P. Schiller,et al.  Quantitative studies of single-cell properties in monkey striate cortex. II. Orientation specificity and ocular dominance. , 1976, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  W. Kohler,et al.  Figural after-effects in the third dimensions of visual space. , 1947, The American journal of psychology.

[14]  J. Ninio Orientational versus Horizontal Disparity in the Stereoscopic Appreciation of Slant , 1985, Perception.

[15]  C. Blakemore,et al.  A second neural mechanism of binocular depth discrimination , 1972, The Journal of physiology.

[16]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex , 1962, The Journal of physiology.

[17]  C. McCollough Color Adaptation of Edge-Detectors in the Human Visual System , 1965, Science.

[18]  B. Rogers,et al.  Anisotropies in the perception of stereoscopic surfaces: The role of orientation disparity , 1993, Vision Research.

[19]  Casper J. Erkelens,et al.  Adaptation to disparity but not to perceived depth , 2001, Vision Research.

[20]  J. Gibson Adaptation, after-effect, and contrast in the perception of tilted lines. II. Simultaneous contrast and the areal restriction of the after-effect. , 1937 .

[21]  P. O. Bishop,et al.  Discrimination of orientation and position disparities by binocularly activated neurons in cat straite cortex. , 1977, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  J. Rice Mathematical Statistics and Data Analysis , 1988 .