On Spatial Cognition and Mobility Strategies

People acquire spatial knowledge by physically experiencing the environment through locomotion. Spatial knowledge generally emerges from the interactions between the specific types of sensory data and the cognitive strategies involved in locomotion. This chapter reviews the fundamental concepts and evidences of spatial cognition. First, the interplay between visual data and mobility is addressed. The importance of landmarks and visual cues is highlighted for implementing both simple and complex navigation strategies such as path integration (PI), landmark-based, and geometry-based, which are the main cognitive mechanisms for spatial learning. Some neural evidences of spatial cognition are given to underline the brain mechanisms involved in cognitive mapping. A review of relevant literature models on cognitive mapping is also presented to better understand how spatial representations are formed at the level of the brain. The chapter provides some insights on how to adapt all these concepts for mobility and spatial knowledge assistance of people with visual impairments.

[1]  Jean Pailhous,et al.  The top down and bottom up mechanisms involved in the sudden awareness of low level sensorimotor behavior. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[2]  A. King,et al.  Multisensory Integration: Strategies for Synchronization , 2005, Current Biology.

[3]  J C Gore,et al.  Differential anterior prefrontal activation during the recognition stage of a spatial working memory task. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[4]  K. Cheng A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation , 1986, Cognition.

[5]  M. Petrides,et al.  Retrosplenial and hippocampal brain regions in human navigation: complementary functional contributions to the formation and use of cognitive maps , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[6]  A. Berthoz Multisensory control of movement , 1993 .

[7]  A. S. Etienne,et al.  A brief view of known landmarks reorientates path integration in hamsters , 2000, Naturwissenschaften.

[8]  Janellen Huttenlocher,et al.  How toddlers represent enclosed spaces , 2003, Cogn. Sci..

[9]  I. Israël,et al.  Vestibular perception of passive whole-body rotation about horizontal and vertical axes in humans: goal-directed vestibulo-ocular reflex and vestibular memory-contingent saccades , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  W. Singer Consciousness and the Binding Problem , 2001, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[11]  Barbara Tversky,et al.  The Cambridge Handbook of Visuospatial Thinking: Functional Significance of Visuospatial Representations , 2005 .

[12]  E. Maguire,et al.  Patterns of hippocampal‐cortical interaction dissociate temporal lobe memory subsystems , 2000, Hippocampus.

[13]  Andrew P. Duchon,et al.  Humans do not switch between path knowledge and landmarks when learning a new environment , 2007, Psychological research.

[14]  Laurie L Bloomfield,et al.  Spatial encoding in mountain chickadees: features overshadow geometry , 2005, Biology Letters.

[15]  Edwige E. Pissaloux,et al.  Towards a Cognitive Model of Human Mobility: An Investigation of Tactile Perception for use in Mobility Devices , 2016, Journal of Navigation.

[16]  Takatsune Kumada,et al.  Visual search and memory search engage extensive overlapping cerebral cortices: an fMRI study , 2004, NeuroImage.

[17]  Ramiro Velazquez,et al.  Image Matching Optimization via Vision and Inertial Data Fusion: Application to Navigation of the Visually Impaired , 2010, Int. J. Image Graph..

[18]  Weimin Mou,et al.  Allocentric and egocentric updating of spatial memories. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[19]  E. Tolman Cognitive maps in rats and men. , 1948, Psychological review.

[20]  J. Konczak Effects of optic flow on the kinematics of human gait: a comparison of young and older adults. , 1994, Journal of motor behavior.

[21]  Roland Maurer,et al.  Resetting the path integrator: a basic condition for route-based navigation , 2004, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[22]  W. Scoville,et al.  LOSS OF RECENT MEMORY AFTER BILATERAL HIPPOCAMPAL LESIONS , 1957, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[23]  Irini Giannopulu,et al.  Spatial linear navigation: Is vision necessary? , 2013, Neuroscience Letters.

[24]  Roland Maurer,et al.  Homing by path integration in a new environment , 2003, Animal Behaviour.

[25]  Claire Leconte,et al.  The plasticity of gravitational reference frame and the subjective vertical: Peripheral visual information affects the oblique effect , 2005, Neuroscience Letters.

[26]  Mark Haselgrove,et al.  Thinking outside of the box: Transfer of shape-based reorientation across the boundary of an arena , 2016, Cognitive Psychology.

[27]  H. Collewijn The vestibulo-ocular reflex: an outdated concept? , 1989, Progress in brain research.

[28]  Jonathan W. Kelly,et al.  Perception of heading without retinal optic flow , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.

[29]  R. Dolan,et al.  The Nose Smells What the Eye Sees Crossmodal Visual Facilitation of Human Olfactory Perception , 2003, Neuron.

[30]  Peter J. Werkhoven,et al.  The Effects of Proprioceptive and Visual Feedback on Geographical Orientation in Virtual Environments , 1999, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[31]  L. Zupan,et al.  Neural processing of gravitoinertial cues in humans. III. Modeling tilt and translation responses. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  Shift of the beating field of vestibular nystagmus: an orientation strategy? , 1998, Neuroscience Letters.

[33]  Roy A. Ruddle,et al.  Effects of proprioceptive feedback and environmental characteristics on spatial learning in virtual environments , 2004, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[34]  Catherine Thinus-Blanc,et al.  Rhesus monkeys use geometric and nongeometric information during a reorientation task , 2001 .

[35]  J. Loomis,et al.  Perspectives on human spatial cognition: memory, navigation, and environmental learning , 2007 .

[36]  R. Wehner,et al.  Look and turn: landmark-based goal navigation in honey bees , 2005, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[37]  Bruce L. McNaughton,et al.  Path integration and the neural basis of the 'cognitive map' , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[38]  Mandyam V. Srinivasan,et al.  Where paths meet and cross: navigation by path integration in the desert ant and the honeybee , 2015, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[39]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Heading in the rat: Determination by environmental shape , 1988 .

[40]  A. Siegel,et al.  The development of spatial representations of large-scale environments. , 1975, Advances in child development and behavior.

[41]  A Berthoz,et al.  The contribution of otoliths and semicircular canals to the perception of two‐dimensional passive whole‐body motion in humans , 1997, The Journal of physiology.

[42]  Hanspeter A. Mallot,et al.  The Role of Global and Local Landmarks in Virtual Environment Navigation , 2000, Presence Teleoperators Virtual Environ..

[43]  L. R. Young,et al.  Influence of combined visual and vestibular cues on human perception and control of horizontal rotation , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[44]  D. Touretzky,et al.  Cognitive maps beyond the hippocampus , 1997, Hippocampus.

[45]  Joseph M. Austen,et al.  Transfer of spatial search between environments in human adults and young children (Homo sapiens): implications for representation of local geometry by spatial systems. , 2014, Developmental psychobiology.

[46]  P. Graham,et al.  What can we learn from studies of insect navigation? , 2012, Animal Behaviour.

[47]  R. Held,et al.  MOVEMENT-PRODUCED STIMULATION IN THE DEVELOPMENT OF VISUALLY GUIDED BEHAVIOR. , 1963, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[48]  John A. King,et al.  Memory for events and their spatial context: models and experiments. , 2001, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[49]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Humans combine the optic flow with static depth cues for robust perception of heading , 1994, Vision Research.

[50]  Laurence R. Young,et al.  Optimal Estimator Model for Human Spatial Orientation a , 1988 .

[51]  M Behrmann,et al.  Active control of locomotion facilitates nonvisual navigation. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[52]  Jacques Droulez,et al.  Stereo-motion cooperation and the use ofmotion disparity in the visual perception of 3-D structure , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[53]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Recalling Routes around London: Activation of the Right Hippocampus in Taxi Drivers , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  S Glasauer Interaction of Semicircular Canals and Otoliths in the Processing Structure of the Subjective Zenith , 1992, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[55]  Lucia F Jacobs,et al.  Unpacking the cognitive map: the parallel map theory of hippocampal function. , 2003, Psychological review.

[56]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Knowing where and getting there: a human navigation network. , 1998, Science.

[57]  Lee Dn,et al.  The optic flow field: the foundation of vision. , 1980 .

[58]  Jan J. Koenderink,et al.  Pappus in optical space , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.

[59]  Li Li,et al.  Humans can perceive heading without visual path information. , 2006, Journal of vision.

[60]  G. Beugnon,et al.  Ant navigation en route to the goal: signature routes facilitate way-finding of Gigantiops destructor , 2006, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[61]  M. Petrides,et al.  Cognitive Strategies Dependent on the Hippocampus and Caudate Nucleus in Human Navigation: Variability and Change with Practice , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[62]  E. Maguire,et al.  A 'landmark' study on the neural basis of navigation , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[63]  Andrew P. Duchon,et al.  Do Humans Integrate Routes Into a Cognitive Map? Map- Versus Landmark-Based Navigation of Novel Shortcuts , 2005 .

[64]  Antoine Wystrach,et al.  Landmarks or panoramas: what do navigating ants attend to for guidance? , 2011, Frontiers in Zoology.

[65]  T. Brandt,et al.  Reciprocal inhibitory visual-vestibular interaction. Visual motion stimulation deactivates the parieto-insular vestibular cortex. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[66]  Martin Egelhaaf,et al.  The Behavioral Relevance of Landmark Texture for Honeybee Homing , 2011, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[67]  V. V. Marlinsky,et al.  Vestibular and vestibulo-proprioceptive perception of motion in the horizontal plane in blindfolded man—III. Route inference , 1999, Neuroscience.

[68]  Jean-Philippe Azulay,et al.  Influence of visual cues on gait in Parkinson's disease: Contribution to attention or sensory dependence? , 2006, Journal of the Neurological Sciences.

[69]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Reference Frames for Spatial Cognition: Different Brain Areas are Involved in Viewer-, Object-, and Landmark-Centered Judgments About Object Location , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[70]  J. Lackner Some proprioceptive influences on the perceptual representation of body shape and orientation. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[71]  Edward H Cornell,et al.  Human non-visual discrimination of gradual turning is poor , 2007, Psychological research.

[72]  Reginald G. Golledge,et al.  The Encoding-Error Model of Pathway Completion without Vision , 2010 .

[73]  M. Spetch,et al.  Use of geometric properties of landmark arrays for reorientation relative to remote cities and local objects. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[74]  R. Müri,et al.  Allocentric and Egocentric Spatial Impairments in a Case of Topographical Disorientation , 2005, Cortex.

[75]  Markus Lappe,et al.  Absolute travel distance from optic flow , 2005, Vision Research.

[76]  Julie M. Harris,et al.  Using visual direction in three-dimensional motion perception , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[77]  B. Gunter,et al.  Patterns of television viewing and of perceptions of hazards to life , 1985 .

[78]  M. Ohmi Egocentric perception through interaction among many sensory systems. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[79]  Weimin Mou,et al.  Intrinsic frames of reference and egocentric viewpoints in scene recognition , 2008, Cognition.

[80]  A. S. Etienne,et al.  Optimizing visual landmarks: two- and three-dimensional minimal landscapes , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[81]  Alain Berthoz,et al.  Reference frames and cognitive strategies during navigation: is the left hippocampal formation involved in the sequential aspects of route memory? , 2003 .

[82]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Nonvisual navigation by blind and sighted: assessment of path integration ability. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[83]  Lars Chittka,et al.  Navigation without vision: bumblebee orientation in complete darkness , 1999, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[84]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Erratum to “Spatial invariance in anticipatory orienting behaviour during human navigation” [Neuroscience Letters 339 (2002) 243–247] , 2004, Neuroscience Letters.

[85]  Aiden E. G. F. Arnold,et al.  A critical review of the allocentric spatial representation and its neural underpinnings: toward a network-based perspective , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[86]  J. O’Keefe,et al.  Geometric determinants of the place fields of hippocampal neurons , 1996, Nature.

[87]  C. Assaiante,et al.  Discrete visual samples may control locomotor equilibrium and foot positioning in man. , 1989, Journal of motor behavior.

[88]  Anthony McGregor,et al.  Absence of an interaction between navigational strategies based on local and distal landmarks. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[89]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  A cortical representation of the local visual environment , 1998, Nature.

[90]  E. D. Haan,et al.  Varieties of human spatial memory: a meta-analysis on the effects of hippocampal lesions , 2001, Brain Research Reviews.

[91]  J. Pearce,et al.  Absence of Overshadowing and Blocking between Landmarks and the Geometric Cues Provided by the Shape of a Test Arena , 2003, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[92]  S. O’Mara,et al.  Hippocampal contributions to neurocognitive mapping in humans: A new model , 2005, Hippocampus.

[93]  N. Burgess Spatial Cognition and the Brain , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[94]  Charlotte Magnusson,et al.  Co-designing together with persons with visual impairments , 2018 .

[95]  R. Jacobs What determines visual cue reliability? , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[96]  C. Oman A heuristic mathematical model for the dynamics of sensory conflict and motion sickness. , 1982, Acta oto-laryngologica. Supplementum.

[97]  E. Save,et al.  Spatial Firing of Hippocampal Place Cells in Blind Rats , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[98]  J. Huttenlocher,et al.  Toddlers' use of metric information and landmarks to reorient. , 2001, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[99]  H. Bülthoff,et al.  Spatial updating in virtual reality: the sufficiency of visual information , 2007, Psychological research.

[100]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Perception of linear horizontal self-motion induced by peripheral vision (linearvection) basic characteristics and visual-vestibular interactions , 1975, Experimental Brain Research.

[101]  David Eilam,et al.  Spatial behavior: the impact of global and local geometry , 2011, Animal Cognition.

[102]  Michael A Motes,et al.  Updating representations of learned scenes , 2007, Psychological research.

[103]  H. Bülthoff,et al.  Merging the senses into a robust percept , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[104]  H. Galiana,et al.  Complex podokinetic (PK) response to post-rotational vestibular stimulation. , 2000, Archives italiennes de biologie.

[105]  A. Wunderlich,et al.  Brain activation during human navigation: gender-different neural networks as substrate of performance , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[106]  S. Doherty,et al.  A conceptual model and empirical analysis of children's acquisition of spatial knowledge , 1985 .

[107]  Christian Büchel,et al.  Neural foundations of emerging route knowledge in complex spatial environments. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[108]  Catherine Thinus-Blanc,et al.  Generalization in place learning and geometry knowledge in rats. , 2004, Learning & memory.

[109]  Bernhard Hommel,et al.  Temporal dynamics of unimodal and multimodal feature binding , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[110]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Spatial orientation in humans: perception of angular whole-body displacements in two-dimensional trajectories , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[111]  N Burgess,et al.  Bilateral hippocampal pathology impairs topographical and episodic memory but not visual pattern matching , 2001, Hippocampus.

[112]  R. Wehner,et al.  Ant Navigation: One-Way Routes Rather Than Maps , 2006, Current Biology.

[113]  Eleanor A. Maguire,et al.  Distinct Neural Systems for the Encoding and Recognition of Topography and Faces , 2001, NeuroImage.

[114]  Christine M Valiquette,et al.  Biased representations of the spatial structure of navigable environments , 2007, Psychological research.

[115]  E. Maguire,et al.  Neurodevelopmental Aspects of Spatial Navigation: A Virtual Reality fMRI Study , 2002, NeuroImage.

[116]  Raymond P. Kesner,et al.  An analysis of independence and interactions of brain substrates that subserve multiple attributes, memory systems, and underlying processes , 2004, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[117]  G. Winocur,et al.  “I have often walked down this street before”: fMRI Studies on the hippocampus and other structures during mental navigation of an old environment , 2004, Hippocampus.

[118]  P J Kellman,et al.  Extracting object motion during observer motion: combining constraints from optic flow and binocular disparity. , 1995, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[119]  Isabelle Israël,et al.  Optic-Flow-Based Perception of Two-Dimensional Trajectories and the Effects of a Single Landmark , 2005, Perception.

[120]  Pascal Prévost,et al.  Spatial invariance in anticipatory orienting behaviour during human navigation , 2003, Neuroscience Letters.

[121]  David I. Anderson,et al.  Responsiveness to terrestrial optic flow in infancy: does locomotor experience play a role? , 2006, Human movement science.

[122]  S. Bricogne,et al.  Neural Correlates of Topographic Mental Exploration: The Impact of Route versus Survey Perspective Learning , 2000, NeuroImage.

[123]  G. Allen,et al.  Developmental issues in cognitive mapping: the selection and utilization of environmental landmarks. , 1979, Child development.

[124]  T. Collett,et al.  Animal Navigation: Path Integration, Visual Landmarks and Cognitive Maps , 2004, Current Biology.

[125]  Horst Mittelstaedt,et al.  Idiothetic navigation in humans: estimation of path length , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[126]  Thomas S. Collett,et al.  Memory use in insect visual navigation , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[127]  J Ward-Robinson,et al.  Influence of a beacon on spatial learning based on the shape of the test environment. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[128]  L. Nadel,et al.  The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map , 1978 .

[129]  Robert Cohen,et al.  The Development of spatial cognition , 1985 .

[130]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[131]  A. Etienne,et al.  Navigation through vector addition , 1998, Nature.

[132]  F. Schenk,et al.  Adaptive changes in a radial maze task: Efficient selection of baited arms with reduced foraging in senescent hooded rats , 2006, Behavioural Brain Research.

[133]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  Visual Homing Is Possible Without Landmarks: A Path Integration Study in Virtual Reality , 2002, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[134]  R. Wehner,et al.  Local vectors in desert ants: context-dependent landmark learning during outbound and homebound runs , 2003, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[135]  Barbara Tversky,et al.  Cognitive Maps, Cognitive Collages, and Spatial Mental Models , 1993, COSIT.

[136]  Sara J Shettleworth,et al.  The geometric module in the rat: independence of shape and feature learning in a food finding task , 2004, Learning & behavior.

[137]  N. Newcombe,et al.  Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? squaring theory and evidence , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[138]  C. Gallistel The organization of learning , 1990 .

[139]  C. R. Gordon,et al.  Adaptive plasticity in the control of locomotor trajectory , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[140]  William H Warren,et al.  Path Integration from Optic Flow and Body Senses in a Homing Task , 2002, Perception.

[141]  J. Taube,et al.  Firing Properties of Head Direction Cells in the Rat Anterior Thalamic Nucleus: Dependence on Vestibular Input , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[142]  N. Gale,et al.  Exploring the anchor-point hypothesis of spatial cognition , 1987 .

[143]  E. Spelke,et al.  Human Spatial Representation: Insights from Animals , 2002 .

[144]  W. Bles,et al.  Differential effects of ambivalent visual-vestibular-somatosensory stimulation on the perception of self-motion , 1985, Behavioural Brain Research.

[145]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  The Parahippocampus Subserves Topographical Learning in Man , 1996 .

[146]  J. O'Keefe,et al.  The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. , 1971, Brain research.

[147]  Ken Cheng,et al.  Reflections on geometry and navigation , 2005, Connect. Sci..

[148]  Jennifer L. Campos,et al.  Multisensory integration in the estimation of relative path length , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[149]  Morris Moscovitch,et al.  Where to? Remote Memory for Spatial Relations and Landmark Identity in Former Taxi Drivers with Alzheimer's Disease and Encephalitis , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[150]  Christopher K. Rhea,et al.  Control of adaptive locomotion: effect of visual obstruction and visual cues in the environment , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[151]  G. Johansson Studies on Visual Perception of Locomotion , 1977, Perception.

[152]  J. Gibson The perception of visual surfaces. , 1950, The American journal of psychology.

[153]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Adaptive modification of the vestibulo-ocular reflex by mental effort in darkness , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[154]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children , 1994, Nature.

[155]  A Berthoz,et al.  Spatial memory of body linear displacement: what is being stored? , 1995, Science.

[156]  Michael S. Landy,et al.  The kinetic depth effect and optic flowȁII. First- and second-order motion , 1991, Vision Research.

[157]  R. Andersen,et al.  Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements. , 1997, Annual review of neuroscience.

[158]  Michel Denis,et al.  Spatial Descriptions as Navigational Aids: A Cognitive Analysis of Route Directions , 1998, Kognitionswissenschaft.

[159]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Dissociating visual and kinesthetic coordinates during pointing movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[160]  E. Maguire,et al.  The Well-Worn Route and the Path Less Traveled Distinct Neural Bases of Route Following and Wayfinding in Humans , 2003, Neuron.

[161]  Stella F. Lourenco,et al.  Using geometry to specify location: implications for spatial coding in children and nonhuman animals , 2007, Psychological research.

[162]  Benjamin Kuipers,et al.  Navigation and Mapping in Large Scale Space , 1988, AI Mag..

[163]  Gabriele Janzen,et al.  Selective neural representation of objects relevant for navigation , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[164]  William H. Warren,et al.  Optic flow is used to control human walking , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[165]  Brice Isableu,et al.  Visual contribution to self-induced body sway frequencies and visual perception of male professional dancers , 1999, Neuroscience Letters.

[166]  D S Touretzky,et al.  Theory of rodent navigation based on interacting representations of space , 1996, Hippocampus.

[167]  Edward M. Hubbard,et al.  Neurocognitive Mechanisms of Synesthesia , 2005, Neuron.

[168]  J. Rieser,et al.  Visual Perception and the Guidance of Locomotion without Vision to Previously Seen Targets , 1990, Perception.

[169]  J J Bloomberg,et al.  Optic flow dominates visual scene polarity in causing adaptive modification of locomotor trajectory. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[170]  R. Golledge,et al.  Spatial Behavior: A Geographic Perspective , 1996 .

[171]  Simon Fleminger,et al.  Allocentric spatial memory in humans with hippocampal lesions. , 2005, Acta psychologica.

[172]  David Waller,et al.  The role of body-based sensory information in the acquisition of enduring spatial representations , 2007, Psychological research.

[173]  A Berthoz,et al.  Spatial memory and path integration studied by self-driven passive linear displacement. I. Basic properties. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[174]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Mental navigation along memorized routes activates the hippocampus, precuneus, and insula , 1997, Neuroreport.

[175]  Stephanie Clarke,et al.  Intrinsic connectivity of human superior colliculus , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[176]  R Sekuler,et al.  Optic Flow Helps Humans Learn to Navigate through Synthetic Environments , 2000, Perception.

[177]  J. O'keefe Cognitive maps in infants? , 1994, Nature.

[178]  Sang Ah Lee,et al.  Navigation as a source of geometric knowledge: Young children’s use of length, angle, distance, and direction in a reorientation task , 2012, Cognition.

[179]  Hanna Damasio,et al.  The neuroanatomical correlates of route learning impairment , 2000, Neuropsychologia.