Infants’ Somatotopic Neural Responses to Seeing Human Actions: I’ve Got You under My Skin

Human infants rapidly learn new skills and customs via imitation, but the neural linkages between action perception and production are not well understood. Neuroscience studies in adults suggest that a key component of imitation–identifying the corresponding body part used in the acts of self and other–has an organized neural signature. In adults, perceiving someone using a specific body part (e.g., hand vs. foot) is associated with activation of the corresponding area of the sensory and/or motor strip in the observer’s brain–a phenomenon called neural somatotopy. Here we examine whether preverbal infants also exhibit somatotopic neural responses during the observation of others’ actions. 14-month-old infants were randomly assigned to watch an adult reach towards and touch an object using either her hand or her foot. The scalp electroencephalogram (EEG) was recorded and event-related changes in the sensorimotor mu rhythm were analyzed. Mu rhythm desynchronization was greater over hand areas of sensorimotor cortex during observation of hand actions and was greater over the foot area for observation of foot actions. This provides the first evidence that infants’ observation of someone else using a particular body part activates the corresponding areas of sensorimotor cortex. We hypothesize that this somatotopic organization in the developing brain supports imitation and cultural learning. The findings connect developmental cognitive neuroscience, adult neuroscience, action representation, and behavioral imitation.

[1]  J. R. Smith The Frequency Growth of the Human Alpha Rhythms During Normal Infancy and Childhood , 1941 .

[2]  I. Hagne Development of the Sleep EEG in Normal Infants During the First Year of Life , 1972 .

[3]  I Hagne,et al.  Development of the EEG in normal infants during the first year of life. A longitudinal study. , 1972, Acta paediatrica Scandinavica. Supplement.

[4]  Aileen B. Sedman,et al.  A longitudinal study , 1987 .

[5]  A. Meltzoff Infant Imitation After a 1-Week Delay: Long-Term Memory for Novel Acts and Multiple Stimuli. , 1988, Developmental psychology.

[6]  S. Makeig Auditory event-related dynamics of the EEG spectrum and effects of exposure to tones. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[7]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[8]  G. Pfurtscheller,et al.  Foot and hand area mu rhythms. , 1997, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[9]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Explaining Facial Imitation: A Theoretical Model. , 1997, Early development & parenting.

[10]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Neuropsychological correlates of early symptoms of autism. , 1998, Child development.

[11]  Elena V Orekhova,et al.  EEG alpha rhythm in infants , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[12]  G. Pfurtscheller,et al.  Functional dissociation of lower and upper frequency mu rhythms in relation to voluntary limb movement , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[13]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[14]  N. Fox,et al.  Development of the EEG from 5 months to 4 years of age , 2002, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[15]  R. Adolphs Cognitive neuroscience: Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[16]  Aina Puce,et al.  Viewing the motion of human body parts activates different regions of premotor, temporal, and parietal cortex , 2004, NeuroImage.

[17]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[18]  J. Pineda The functional significance of mu rhythms: Translating “seeing” and “hearing” into “doing” , 2005, Brain Research Reviews.

[19]  S-J Blakemore,et al.  Somatosensory activations during the observation of touch and a case of vision-touch synaesthesia. , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[20]  G. Dawson,et al.  Early Predictors of Communication Development in Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: Joint Attention, Imitation, and Toy Play , 2006, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[21]  Sally J. Rogers,et al.  Imitation and the Social Mind: Autism and Typical Development. , 2006 .

[22]  Claes von Hofsten,et al.  Action in development. , 2007 .

[23]  C. Frith,et al.  Social Cognition in Humans , 2007, Current Biology.

[24]  A. Meltzoff 'Like me': a foundation for social cognition. , 2007, Developmental science.

[25]  Yehezkel Ben-Ari,et al.  Rapid cortical oscillations and early motor activity in premature human neonate. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[26]  F. H. Lopes da Silva,et al.  Short‐lived brain state after cued motor imagery in naive subjects , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.

[27]  Atsushi Iriki,et al.  Social cognition in premotor and parietal cortex , 2008, Social neuroscience.

[28]  R. Hari,et al.  Brain basis of human social interaction: from concepts to brain imaging. , 2009, Physiological reviews.

[29]  Mark H Johnson,et al.  Predictive motor activation during action observation in human infants , 2009, Biology Letters.

[30]  Terrence J Sejnowski,et al.  Foundations for a New Science of Learning , 2009, Science.

[31]  Janny C. Stapel,et al.  Motor activation during observation of unusual versus ordinary actions in infancy , 2010, Social neuroscience.

[32]  Dominique L. Pritchett,et al.  Cued Spatial Attention Drives Functionally Relevant Modulation of the Mu Rhythm in Primary Somatosensory Cortex , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[34]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Motor System Activation Reveals Infants’ On-Line Prediction of Others’ Goals , 2010, Psychological science.

[35]  Bin He,et al.  Negative covariation between task-related responses in alpha/beta-band activity and BOLD in human sensorimotor cortex: An EEG and fMRI study of motor imagery and movements , 2010, NeuroImage.

[36]  P. Gerrans Mirror neuron systems: The role of mirroring processes in social cognition , 2010 .

[37]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Neural mirroring systems: Exploring the EEG mu rhythm in human infancy , 2011, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[38]  Marco Iacoboni,et al.  Neural correlates of dyadic interaction during infancy , 2011, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[39]  Kerstin Rosander,et al.  Using mu rhythm desynchronization to measure mirror neuron activity in infants. , 2011, Developmental science.

[40]  M. Berchicci,et al.  Development of Mu Rhythm in Infants and Preschool Children , 2011, Developmental Neuroscience.

[41]  Andrew N Meltzoff,et al.  Neural correlates of action observation and execution in 14-month-old infants: an event-related EEG desynchronization study. , 2011, Developmental science.

[42]  Andrew N Meltzoff,et al.  Neural correlates of being imitated: An EEG study in preverbal infants , 2012, Social neuroscience.

[43]  Joni N. Saby,et al.  The Utility of EEG Band Power Analysis in the Study of Infancy and Early Childhood , 2012, Developmental neuropsychology.

[44]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  11 Developmental Perspectives on Action Science: Lessons from Infant Imitation and Cognitive Neuroscience , 2013 .

[45]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  Action science: Foundations of an emerging discipline , 2013 .

[46]  Jan R. Wiersema,et al.  Infants' mu suppression during the observation of real and mimicked goal-directed actions. , 2013, Developmental science.

[47]  Joni N. Saby,et al.  Infant Brain Responses to Object Weight: Exploring Goal-Directed Actions and Self-Experience. , 2013, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.