Solving future problems: Default network and executive activity associated with goal-directed mental simulations

Mental simulations are often focused on a goal in the future or a problem to be solved. Recent neuroimaging studies have associated mental simulations of the future with default network activity, but the simulations in these studies were not typically directed toward achieving a particular goal. Goal-directed simulation requires cognitive control to maintain information, make decisions, and coordinate abstract action sequences. Therefore, it should recruit not only the default network, but also executive regions. To investigate whether default network and executive regions can be coactive in the context of goal-directed simulation, we designed a problem-solving task in which participants simulated solving several specific problems in imaginary scenarios while in the MRI scanner. We analyzed brain activity during simulation relative to a semantic elaboration task and found that goal-directed simulation engaged core regions of the default network and executive dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. A functional connectivity analysis with posterior cingulate and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex seeds revealed that activity in these regions was coupled throughout the goal-directed simulation period and associated with a distributed network of other default and executive regions, including medial prefrontal cortex, medial temporal, and parietal regions.

[1]  T. Paus,et al.  Functional coactivation map of the human brain. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[2]  Keith A. Johnson,et al.  Cortical Hubs Revealed by Intrinsic Functional Connectivity: Mapping, Assessment of Stability, and Relation to Alzheimer's Disease , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  R. Seitz,et al.  Value judgments and self-control of action: The role of the medial frontal cortex , 2009, Brain Research Reviews.

[4]  D. Schacter,et al.  Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[5]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Common Blood Flow Changes across Visual Tasks: II. Decreases in Cerebral Cortex , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  From Conditioning to Conscious Recollection , 2001 .

[7]  Colleen M. Seifert,et al.  Opportunistic Planning: Being Reminded of Pending Goals , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[8]  E. Koechlin,et al.  The Architecture of Cognitive Control in the Human Prefrontal Cortex , 2003, Science.

[9]  K. Szpunar,et al.  Contextual processing in episodic future thought. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[10]  Vinod Menon,et al.  Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[11]  G. Shulman,et al.  Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[12]  Steven Laureys,et al.  Cytology and functionally correlated circuits of human posterior cingulate areas , 2006, NeuroImage.

[13]  Anthony Randal McIntosh,et al.  A common functional brain network for autobiographical, episodic, and semantic memory retrieval , 2010, NeuroImage.

[14]  D. Schacter,et al.  The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: remembering the past and imagining the future , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[15]  Bryan T. Denny,et al.  Medial prefrontal activity differentiates self from close others. , 2006, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[16]  Steve Majerus,et al.  The Neural Basis of Personal Goal Processing When Envisioning Future Events , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[18]  N. Cohen From Conditioning to Conscious Recollection Memory Systems of the Brain. Oxford Psychology Series, Volume 35. , 2001 .

[19]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Medial prefrontal cortex subserves diverse forms of self-reflection , 2011, Social neuroscience.

[20]  A. McIntosh,et al.  Mapping cognition to the brain through neural interactions. , 1999, Memory.

[21]  R. Turner,et al.  Event-Related fMRI: Characterizing Differential Responses , 1998, NeuroImage.

[22]  R. Buckner,et al.  Self-projection and the brain , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[23]  R. Buckner,et al.  Functional-Anatomic Fractionation of the Brain's Default Network , 2010, Neuron.

[24]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network, supports goal-directed cognition , 2010, NeuroImage.

[25]  J. Binder,et al.  A Parametric Manipulation of Factors Affecting Task-induced Deactivation in Functional Neuroimaging , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Is the rostro-caudal axis of the frontal lobe hierarchical? , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[27]  Perrine Ruby,et al.  Distinct Regions of the Medial Prefrontal Cortex Are Associated with Self-referential Processing and Perspective Taking , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[28]  D. Norman,et al.  Attention to action: Willed and automatic control , 1980 .

[29]  M. Fox,et al.  The global signal and observed anticorrelated resting state brain networks. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  William W. Graves,et al.  Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[31]  K. A. Hadland,et al.  Role of the human medial frontal cortex in task switching: a combined fMRI and TMS study. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  Edward M. Bowden,et al.  The Prepared Mind , 2006, Psychological science.

[33]  R. Nathan Spreng,et al.  The Common Neural Basis of Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, Navigation, Theory of Mind, and the Default Mode: A Quantitative Meta-analysis , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  Georg Northoff,et al.  Self-referential processing in our brain—A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self , 2006, NeuroImage.

[35]  R. Buckner,et al.  Evidence for the Default Network's Role in Spontaneous Cognition , 2010 .

[36]  Sterling C. Johnson,et al.  Relevance to self: A brief review and framework of neural systems underlying appraisal , 2007, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[37]  Bharat B. Biswal,et al.  Competition between functional brain networks mediates behavioral variability , 2008, NeuroImage.

[38]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Wandering Minds: The Default Network and Stimulus-Independent Thought , 2007, Science.

[39]  G. Schwartz,et al.  Consciousness and Self-Regulation , 1976 .

[40]  R. Nathan Spreng,et al.  Patterns of Brain Activity Supporting Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, and Theory of Mind, and Their Relationship to the Default Mode Network , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  Alana T. Wong,et al.  Remembering the past and imagining the future: Common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[42]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory , 1995, Nature.

[43]  D. Schacter,et al.  The Brain's Default Network , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[44]  P. Fransson Spontaneous low‐frequency BOLD signal fluctuations: An fMRI investigation of the resting‐state default mode of brain function hypothesis , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[45]  D. Schacter,et al.  Ageing and the self-reference effect in memory , 2007, Memory.

[46]  C. Frith,et al.  Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[47]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[48]  R. Buckner The role of the hippocampus in prediction and imagination. , 2010, Annual review of psychology.

[49]  O. Sporns,et al.  Mapping the Structural Core of Human Cerebral Cortex , 2008, PLoS biology.

[50]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[51]  D. Schacter,et al.  Functional neuroimaging of self-referential encoding with age , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[52]  Todd B. Parrish,et al.  A Brain Mechanism for Facilitation of Insight by Positive Affect , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[53]  Michelle Hampson,et al.  Functional connectivity between task-positive and task-negative brain areas and its relation to working memory performance. , 2010, Magnetic resonance imaging.

[54]  Martial Van der Linden,et al.  Self-referential reflective activity and its relationship with rest: a PET study , 2005, NeuroImage.

[55]  Matthew Flatt,et al.  PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers , 1993 .

[56]  Kevin Murphy,et al.  The impact of global signal regression on resting state correlations: Are anti-correlated networks introduced? , 2009, NeuroImage.

[57]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Functional neuroimaging of selfreferential encoding with age , 2010 .

[58]  Masatoshi Itoh,et al.  Thinking of the future and past: the roles of the frontal pole and the medial temporal lobes , 2003, NeuroImage.

[59]  Jane F. Banfield,et al.  Medial prefrontal activity predicts memory for self. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[60]  K. Christoff,et al.  Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind wandering , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[61]  Jeffrey W. Cooney,et al.  Hierarchical cognitive control deficits following damage to the human frontal lobe , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[62]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Evidence for a frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[63]  Robert Tibshirani,et al.  The Bootstrap Method for Assessing Statistical Accuracy , 1985 .

[64]  D. Norman,et al.  Attention to Action: Willed and Automatic Control of Behavior Technical Report No. 8006. , 1980 .

[65]  M. Linden,et al.  Frequency, characteristics and functions of future‐oriented thoughts in daily life , 2011 .

[66]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[67]  P. Gollwitzer Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. , 1999 .

[68]  K. Szpunar,et al.  Neural substrates of envisioning the future , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[69]  D. Schacter,et al.  On the nature of medial temporal lobe contributions to the constructive simulation of future events , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[70]  D. Schacter,et al.  Episodic Simulation of Future Events , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[71]  Dominic J. Packer,et al.  Neural correlates of reflection on goal states: The role of regulatory focus and temporal distance , 2009, Social neuroscience.

[72]  D. Schacter,et al.  Constructive episodic simulation of the future and the past: Distinct subsystems of a core brain network mediate imagining and remembering , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[73]  M. Raichle,et al.  Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[74]  Edward M. Bowden,et al.  The origins of insight in resting-state brain activity , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[75]  A. R. McIntosh,et al.  Spatiotemporal analysis of event-related fMRI data using partial least squares , 2004, NeuroImage.