Dual-inheritance theory: The evolution of human cultural capacities and cultural evolution

In the early 1930's Wintrop and Luella Kellogg (1933) began co-rearing their 10.5-month-old son, Donald, with a 7.5-month-old female chimpanzee named Gua. The Kelloggs expected that Gua, with the chimpanzee's popular reputa­ tion for aping, would acquire numerous behaviors and practices via imitation from both Donald and themselves. Unexpectedly, however, while Gua did finally acquire a few human patterns (e.g. combing his hair), Donald was the one who began to imitate the chimpanzee in some dramatic ways. Following Gua, Donald acquired the habits of knuckle walking (which he contin­ ued well after achieving full bipedality), chewing on shoes, scraping his teeth against interior walls, and hard biting. Donald even adopted some stereotypical chimpanzee food grunts, barks and hoots, using a particular bark as the word for orange. Thus, it was the human who did most of the aping. People in many small-scale societies believe that a human fetus is formed by many repeated ejaculations of sperm into the womb. This belief means that a child can have multiple fathers, who share paternity according to the number of times they had sex with the mother prior to birth (in anthropological parlance, 'partible paternity'). In response to this cultural belief, women in many of these societies actively seek out extra­ marital copulations, often to provide their child with extra fathers. And, while male jealously from the husband is sometimes a problem, it is regarded as socially inappropriate and thus suppressed. Detailed statistical analyses from two such societies, the Bad of Venezuela (Beckerman et aI., 2002) and Ache of Paraguay (Hill and Hurtado. 1996), show that the optimal number of fathers for a child's survival is more than one. These 'other fathers' (non-husbands of mom) provide resources, in the form of fish and meat, to their offspring and the mothers, both during pregnancy and while the child is growing up. Interestingly, since much of the sex associated with 'extra fathers' occurs after conception, many of these social fathers cannot be the genetic fathers. Culturally transmitted beliefs in partible paternity have been recorded in various linguistically unrelated societies across lowland

[1]  S. Stack Celebrities and suicide: a taxonomy and analysis, 1948-1983. , 1987, American sociological review.

[2]  W. Labov The social origins of sound change , 1979 .

[3]  Jörg Oechssler,et al.  Imitation - Theory and Experimental Evidence , 2003, J. Econ. Theory.

[4]  P. Richerson,et al.  Not by Genes Alone , 2004 .

[5]  L. Cavalli-Sforza Cultural transmission and evolution , 1981 .

[6]  Rita Astuti Comment on F. J. Gil-White's article 'Are ethnic groups biological "species" to the human brain? Essentialism in our cognition of some social categories' , 2001 .

[7]  Julie C. Coultas,et al.  When in Rome... An Evolutionary Perspective on Conformity , 2004 .

[8]  Marian Perkins,et al.  The Nurture Assumption: Why Children Turn Out the Way They Do , 2000, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[9]  Jörgen W. Weibull,et al.  Evolutionary Game Theory , 1996 .

[10]  A. Brooks,et al.  The revolution that wasn't: a new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. , 2000, Journal of human evolution.

[11]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Exact and Approximate Arithmetic in an Amazonian Indigene Group , 2004, Science.

[12]  M. Ghiselin,et al.  Coevolution: Genes, Culture, and Human Diversity , 1991, Politics and the Life Sciences.

[13]  J. Henrich Demography and Cultural Evolution: How Adaptive Cultural Processes Can Produce Maladaptive Losses—The Tasmanian Case , 2004, American Antiquity.

[14]  J. Henrich,et al.  Are Peasants Risk‐Averse Decision Makers?1 , 2002, Current Anthropology.

[15]  Howard M. Weiss,et al.  Influence of modeling on self‐set goals: Direct and mediated effects , 1999 .

[16]  V. Rao Poverty and Public Celebrations in Rural India , 2001 .

[17]  J. Harris Where is the child's environment? A group socialization theory of development. , 1995 .

[18]  G. D. Berreman Hindus of the Himalayas;: Ethnography and change, , 1972 .

[19]  P. Richerson,et al.  Culture and the Evolutionary Process , 1988 .

[20]  S. Stack,et al.  The effect of the media on suicide: evidence from Japan, 1955-1985. , 1996, Suicide & life-threatening behavior.

[21]  Yvon Keromnes,et al.  Space in Language and Cognition , 2007 .

[22]  I. Wasserman,et al.  Suicide and the Media: The New York Times's Presentation of Front‐Page Suicide Stories Between 1910 and 1920 , 1994 .

[23]  Michael X Cohen,et al.  Does similarity breed cooperation? Authors' reply , 2002 .

[24]  Timothy M. Waring,et al.  Article in Press Evolution and Human Behavior Xxx (2005) Xxx – Xxx , 2022 .

[25]  R. Kessler,et al.  Clustering of teenage suicides after television news stories about suicides: a reconsideration. , 1988, The American journal of psychiatry.

[26]  D. Rubinstein Epidemic suicide among Micronesian adolescents. , 1983, Social science & medicine.

[27]  T. Kameda,et al.  Cost-benefit analysis of social/cultural learning 1 Cost-Benefit Analysis of Social/Cultural Learning in a Non-Stationary Uncertain Environment: An Evolutionary Simulation and an Experiment with Human Subjects , 2002 .

[28]  Rick L. Riolo,et al.  Behavioural evolution (Communication arising): Does similarity breed cooperation? , 2002, Nature.

[29]  P. Gordon Numerical Cognition Without Words: Evidence from Amazonia , 2004, Science.

[30]  D. Campbell,et al.  The influence of culture on visual perception , 1967 .

[31]  J. Diamond Guns, Germs, and Steel: The Fates of Human Societies , 1999 .

[32]  L. DeBruine Facial resemblance enhances trust , 2002, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[33]  A. Whiten,et al.  Conformity to cultural norms of tool use in chimpanzees , 2005, Nature.

[34]  W. N. Kellogg,et al.  The Ape and the Child: A study of environmental influence upon early behavior , 1967 .

[35]  F. Barth,et al.  Ethnic Groups and Boundaries by , 2004 .

[36]  M. Tomasello The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition , 2000 .

[37]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Foundations of Human Sociality - Economic Experiments and Ethnographic: Evidence From Fifteen Small-Scale Societies , 2004 .

[38]  K. Jonas Modelling and suicide: a test of the Werther effect. , 1992, The British journal of social psychology.

[39]  E. Rogers Diffusion of Innovations , 1962 .

[40]  B. Mullen,et al.  Jaywalking as a Function of Model Behavior , 1990 .

[41]  Robert S. Baron,et al.  The forgotten variable in conformity research: Impact of task importance on social influence. , 1996 .

[42]  R. Nisbett The geography of thought : how Asians and Westerners think differently--and why , 2003 .

[43]  P. Boyer Religion Explained: The Evolutionary Origins of Religious Thought , 2001 .

[44]  H. Weiss Subordinate imitation of supervisor behavior: The role of modeling in organizational socialization , 1977 .

[45]  Jean E. Jackson,et al.  The Fish People: Linguistic Exogamy and Tukanoan Identity in Northwest Amazonia , 1983 .

[46]  A. Nave Marriage and the maintenance of ethnic group boundaries: the case of Mauritius , 2000 .

[47]  S. Stack Divorce, Suicide, and the Mass Media: An Analysis of Differential Identification, 1948-1980. , 1990 .

[48]  L L Cavalli-Sforza,et al.  Theory and observation in cultural transmission. , 1982, Science.

[49]  J. Henrich,et al.  The evolution of prestige: freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. , 2001, Evolution and human behavior : official journal of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society.

[50]  R. Boyd,et al.  The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence of between-group differences. , 1998 .

[51]  A. Whiten,et al.  Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens) , 2005, Animal Cognition.

[52]  R. Mcelreath,et al.  Shared Norms and the Evolution of Ethnic Markers , 2003, Current Anthropology.

[53]  William C. Terry,et al.  Historical Dynamics: Why States Rise and Fall , 2007 .

[54]  J. Sonnemans,et al.  Learning by experience and learning by imitating successful others , 1998 .

[55]  R. Boyd,et al.  Are Peasants Risk-Averse Decision Makers ? , 2022 .

[56]  J. Henrich,et al.  Why Humans Cooperate: A Cultural and Evolutionary Explanation , 2007 .

[57]  Mark Pingle,et al.  Imitation versus rationality: An experimental perspective on decision making , 1995 .

[58]  David J. Boyd Life Without Pigs: Recent Subsistence Changes Among the Irakia Awa, Papua New Guinea , 2001 .

[59]  F. Gil-White,et al.  Are Ethnic Groups Biological “Species” to the Human Brain? , 2001, Current Anthropology.

[60]  M. Gurven Does Market Exposure Affect Economic Game Behavior , 2004 .

[61]  Michael R. Dove,et al.  Uncertainty, Humility and Adaptation to the Tropical Forest: The Agricultural Augury of the Kantu , 1993 .

[62]  M. Feldman,et al.  Niche construction, biological evolution, and cultural change , 2000, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[63]  Peter J. Richerson,et al.  Why does culture increase human adaptability , 1995 .

[64]  Jeffrey P. Carpenter,et al.  When in Rome: conformity and the provision of public goods , 2004 .

[65]  S. Shennan Demography and Cultural Innovation: a Model and its Implications for the Emergence of Modern Human Culture , 2001, Cambridge Archaeological Journal.

[66]  Haim Levy,et al.  Further Tests of the Separation Theorem and the Capital Asset Pricing Model , 1992 .

[67]  A. Dickson On Evolution , 1884, Science.

[68]  Geoffrey Hall,et al.  Animal cognition , 1985, Nature.

[69]  D. Nettle Social Markers and the Evolution of Reciprocal Exchange , 1997, Current Anthropology.

[70]  R. Thornhill,et al.  Major histocompatibility complex genes, symmetry, and body scent attractiveness in men and women , 2003 .