Looking for intoolligence: A unified framework for the cognitive study of human tool use and technology.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] R. Cubelli,et al. Cognition in Action: Testing a Model of Limb Apraxia , 2000, Brain and Cognition.
[2] G. Goldenberg,et al. Relationship between Object-Related Gestures and the Fractionated Object Knowledge System , 2007, Behavioural neurology.
[3] Norman W. Park,et al. Dissociating the memory systems mediating complex tool knowledge and skills , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[4] A. Glenberg,et al. What memory is for: Creating meaning in the service of action , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[5] Alex Thornton,et al. Cognitive requirements of cumulative culture: teaching is useful but not essential , 2015, Scientific Reports.
[6] B. Beck. Animal Tool Behavior: The Use and Manufacture of Tools by Animals , 1980 .
[7] Derek C. Penn,et al. Darwin's mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds , 2008, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[8] G. Goldenberg. Pantomime of object use: a challenge to cerebral localization of cognitive function , 2003, NeuroImage.
[9] S. Kirby,et al. Cultural evolution of systematically structured behaviour in a non-human primate , 2014, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[10] C. Hall,et al. Chapter 11 Limb Apraxia: A Process Approach , 1992 .
[11] Erminio Capitani,et al. Gender Affects Word Retrieval of Certain Categories in Semantic Fluency Tasks , 1999, Cortex.
[12] M. J. Emerson,et al. The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.
[13] François Osiurak,et al. Tool use disorders after left brain damage , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[14] Mike Hansell,et al. Setting tool use within the context of animal construction behaviour. , 2008, Trends in ecology & evolution.
[15] M. Silveri,et al. Semantic memory in object use , 2009, Neuropsychologia.
[16] Derek C. Penn,et al. Causal cognition in human and nonhuman animals: a comparative, critical review. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.
[17] L. Buxbaum,et al. The Role of Semantic Memory in Object Use , 1997 .
[18] T. Breuer,et al. First Observation of Tool Use in Wild Gorillas , 2005, PLoS biology.
[19] Christine A Caldwell,et al. Social Learning Mechanisms and Cumulative Cultural Evolution , 2009, Psychological science.
[20] G. Goldenberg. Apraxia – The cognitive side of motor control , 2013, Cortex.
[21] Lucia Riggio,et al. Sentence comprehension and simulation of object temporary, canonical and stable affordances , 2009, Brain Research.
[22] G. Gigli,et al. Degraded Semantic Knowledge And Accurate Object Use , 2007, Cortex.
[23] F. Osiurak,et al. Grasping the affordances, understanding the reasoning: toward a dialectical theory of human tool use. , 2010, Psychological review.
[24] A Bartolo,et al. Pantomimes are special gestures which rely on working memory , 2003, Brain and Cognition.
[25] E. Warrington,et al. Categories of knowledge. Further fractionations and an attempted integration. , 1987, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[26] F. Osiurak,et al. Apraxia of tool use: More evidence for the technical reasoning hypothesis , 2013, Cortex.
[27] G. Humphreys,et al. Modelling direct perceptual constraints on action selection: The Naming and Action Model (NAM) , 2002 .
[28] S. Shettleworth. Cognition, evolution, and behavior , 1998 .
[29] S. Thompson-Schill,et al. Manual Experience Shapes Object Representations , 2013, Psychological science.
[30] What’s so special about using tools? , 1987 .
[31] Erminio Capitani,et al. Semantic Category Dissociations, Familiarity and Gender * * Part of this study was presented at the Seventeenth Workshop on Cognitive Neuropsychology, Bressanone, Italy, 24-29 January 1999. , 2000, Cortex.
[32] D. Rosenbaum,et al. Time course of movement planning: selection of handgrips for object manipulation. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[33] Atsushi Iriki,et al. Shaping multisensory action–space with tools: evidence from patients with cross-modal extinction , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[34] E. Capitani,et al. Do living and nonliving categories need further fractionation? A study of picture naming in a pathological sample. , 2000, Brain and cognition.
[35] R. Baillargeon,et al. The Development of Young Infants' Intuitions about Support , 1992 .
[36] M. Rieger. Automatic keypress activation in skilled typing. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[37] F. Osiurak,et al. Unusual use of objects after unilateral brain damage. The technical reasoning model , 2009, Cortex.
[38] C. Lumsden. Culture and the Evolutionary Process, Robert Boyd, Peter J. Richerson. University of Chicago Press, Chicago & London (1985), viii, +301. Price $29.95 , 1986 .
[39] Laurel J Buxbaum,et al. Learning, Remembering, and Predicting How to Use Tools: Distributed Neurocognitive Mechanisms Comment on Osiurak and Badets (2016) , 2017, Psychological review.
[40] C. Heyes,et al. Science Perspectives on Psychological Mirror Neuron Forum on Behalf Of: Association for Psychological Science , 2022 .
[41] K M Heilman,et al. Ideational apraxia--a re-definition. , 1973, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[42] G. Humphreys,et al. The involvement of the dorsal stream in processing implied actions between paired objects: A TMS study , 2017, Neuropsychologia.
[43] Benjamin Naumann. The Architecture Of Cognition , 2016 .
[44] T. Shallice. Specific impairments of planning. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[45] Role of manipulation knowledge in routine tool use , 2014, Cortex.
[46] G. Goldenberg,et al. Tool use and mechanical problem solving in apraxia , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[47] Jamie L. Emerson,et al. Real-life driving outcomes in Parkinson disease , 2011, Neurology.
[48] K M Heilman,et al. Joint coordination deficits in limb apraxia. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[49] Katrin Amunts,et al. The human inferior parietal cortex: Cytoarchitectonic parcellation and interindividual variability , 2006, NeuroImage.
[50] J. Hermsdörfer,et al. It takes the whole brain to make a cup of coffee: the neuropsychology of naturalistic actions involving technical devices , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[51] D. Norman,et al. Attention to Action: Willed and Automatic Control of Behavior Technical Report No. 8006. , 1980 .
[52] Norman W. Park,et al. Interaction of memory systems during acquisition of tool knowledge and skills in Parkinson's disease , 2015, Neuropsychologia.
[53] Amy Needham,et al. Intuitions about support in 4.5-month-old infants , 1993, Cognition.
[54] Gilles Dietrich,et al. The role of expertise in tool use: skill differences in functional action adaptations to task constraints. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[55] Guy A. Orban,et al. The neural basis of human tool use , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[56] K. Heilman,et al. A Cognitive Neuropsychological Model of Limb Praxis , 1991 .
[57] K. Duncker,et al. On problem-solving , 1945 .
[58] A. Simmons. Distributed Practice and Procedural Memory Consolidation in Musicians’ Skill Learning , 2012 .
[59] L. Barsalou. Grounded cognition. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.
[60] F. Osiurak,et al. Handing a tool to someone can take more time than using it , 2013, Cognition.
[61] F. Osiurak,et al. Object utilization and object usage: A single-case study , 2008, Neurocase.
[62] D. Povinelli. Folk physics for apes , 2000 .
[63] D. Spalding. The Principles of Psychology , 1873, Nature.
[64] T Shallice,et al. The domain of supervisory processes and temporal organization of behaviour. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[65] J. Hodges,et al. When objects lose their meaning: What happens to their use? , 2002, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[66] Norman W. Park,et al. Effects of dividing attention on memory for declarative and procedural aspects of tool use , 2016, Memory & cognition.
[67] Ronald R. Peeters,et al. Functional properties of the left parietal tool use region , 2013, NeuroImage.
[68] G. Goldenberg,et al. The Impact of Dysexecutive Syndrome on Use of Tools and Technical Devices , 2007, Cortex.
[69] Stephen J. Lycett,et al. Animal Tool Behavior: The Use and Manufacture of Tools by Animals , 2013 .
[70] Tools , 2019, Oncology Issues.
[71] K M Heilman,et al. Conceptual apraxia from lateralized lesions , 1997, Neurology.
[72] Jeffrey B Wagman,et al. Haptically creating affordances: the user-tool interface. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.
[73] F. Osiurak. What Neuropsychology Tells us About Human Tool Use? The Four Constraints Theory (4CT): Mechanics, Space, Time, and Effort , 2014, Neuropsychology Review.
[74] M. Tomasello,et al. Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[75] G. Goldenberg,et al. The neural basis of tool use. , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[76] E. Biryukova,et al. Organization of goal-directed action at a high level of motor skill: the case of stone knapping in India. , 2008, Motor control.
[77] N. Maier. Reasoning in humans. I. On direction. , 1930 .
[78] Harold Bekkering,et al. Action semantics: A unifying conceptual framework for the selective use of multimodal and modality-specific object knowledge. , 2014, Physics of life reviews.
[79] C. Marsden,et al. Disturbance of sequential movements in patients with Parkinson's disease. , 1987, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[80] François Osiurak,et al. Tool use and affordance: Manipulation-based versus reasoning-based approaches. , 2016, Psychological review.
[81] F. Osiurak,et al. Apraxia and Alzheimer’s Disease: Review and Perspectives , 2013, Neuropsychology Review.
[82] S. Anderson,et al. Road safety in drivers with Parkinson disease , 2009, Neurology.
[83] Dietmar Heinke,et al. Implied actions between paired objects lead to affordance selection by inhibition. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[84] F. Osiurak,et al. Re-examining the gesture engram hypothesis. New perspectives on apraxia of tool use , 2011, Neuropsychologia.
[85] Caruana Fausto,et al. Types of abduction in tool behavior , 2017 .
[86] M. Hegarty. Mechanical reasoning by mental simulation , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[87] M. Grossman,et al. Everyday Action Impairment in Parkinson's Disease Dementia , 2012, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.
[88] E. Capitani,et al. Semantic category dissociations in naming : is there a gender effect in Alzheimer’s disease? , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[89] B. Beck. Animal Tool Behavior , 1980 .
[90] M. L. Lambon Ralph,et al. The role of conceptual knowledge in object use evidence from semantic dementia. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[91] Lynette A. Hart,et al. Cognitive behaviour in Asian elephants: use and modification of branches for fly switching , 2001, Animal Behaviour.
[92] G. Humphreys,et al. The Role of Semantic Knowledge and Working Memory in Everyday Tasks , 2000, Brain and Cognition.
[93] Georg Goldenberg Sonja Hagmann,et al. Therapy of Activities of Daily Living in Patients with Apraxia , 1998 .
[94] E. Renzi,et al. Modality-specific and supramodal mechanisms of apraxia. , 1982, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[95] G. Goldenberg,et al. Assessment and therapy of complex activities of daily living in apraxia , 2001 .
[96] R. Baillargeon,et al. Is the Top Object Adequately Supported by the Bottom Object? Young Infants' Understanding of Support Relations , 1990 .
[97] T. Shallice,et al. Modality-Specific Operations in Semantic Dementia , 1997, Cortex.
[98] F. Osiurak,et al. Mechanical problem-solving strategies in left-brain damaged patients and apraxia of tool use , 2013, Neuropsychologia.
[99] Georg Goldenberg,et al. Defective pantomime of object use in left brain damage: apraxia or asymbolia? , 2003, Neuropsychologia.
[100] K. Heilman,et al. Two forms of ideomotor apraxia , 1982, Neurology.
[101] K. Laland,et al. Experimental Evidence for the Co-Evolution of Hominin Tool-Making Teaching and Language , 2014, Nature Communications.
[102] Emanuelle Reynaud,et al. On the neurocognitive origins of human tool use : A critical review of neuroimaging data , 2016, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[103] F. Osiurak,et al. Physical intelligence does matter to cumulative technological culture. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[104] F. Osiurak. What is the future for tool-specific generalized motor programs? , 2017 .