Sequential effects of high and low guidance on children's early science learning

We describe a microgenetic approach aimed at examining the effect of different sequences of high vs low levels of instructional guidance on students' learning about concepts and procedures associated with simple experimental design - often called the "Control of Variables Strategy (CVS). Third-grade children were randomly assigned to one of four conditions in which CVS was taught via one of the four possible orderings of high or low instructional guidance: (High + High, High + Low, Low + High, and Low + Low). High guidance consisted of a combination of direct instruction and inquiry questions, whereas low guidance was comprised only of inquiry questions. Contrary to commonly held beliefs that high levels of guidance, and in particular, direct instruction, lead only to shallow learning, results show that repeated instances of high instructional guidance were advantageous for both learning and transfer of CVS. Moreover, the High + High group continued to demonstrate strong conceptual understanding of CVS relative to other groups five months after training. Overall the study suggests that strong amounts of instructional guidance are capable of exhibiting powerful effects on children's early science learning.

[1]  D. Klahr,et al.  All other things being equal: acquisition and transfer of the control of variables strategy. , 1999, Child development.

[2]  Daniel L. Schwartz,et al.  Inventing to Prepare for Future Learning: The Hidden Efficiency of Encouraging Original Student Production in Statistics Instruction , 2004 .

[3]  Mark A. McDaniel,et al.  Discovery Learning and Transfer of Problem-Solving Skills , 1990 .

[4]  Richard E. Clark,et al.  Why Minimal Guidance During Instruction Does Not Work: An Analysis of the Failure of Constructivist, Discovery, Problem-Based, Experiential, and Inquiry-Based Teaching , 2006 .

[5]  D. Klahr,et al.  Bridging Research and Practice: A Cognitively Based Classroom Intervention for Teaching Experimentation Skills to Elementary School Children , 2000 .

[6]  Slava Kalyuga Expertise Reversal Effect and Its Implications for Learner-Tailored Instruction , 2007 .

[7]  Daniel L. Schwartz,et al.  A time for telling , 1998 .

[8]  Manu Kapur Productive Failure , 2006, ICLS.

[9]  David Klahr,et al.  Cognitive Research and Elementary Science Instruction: From the Laboratory, to the Classroom, and Back , 2005 .

[10]  Kevin Crowley,et al.  The microgenetic method. A direct means for studying cognitive development. , 1991, The American psychologist.

[11]  Lara M. Triona,et al.  Hands on What? The Relative Effectiveness of Physical versus Virtual Materials in an Engineering Design Project by Middle School Children , 2007 .

[12]  D. Kuhn Strategies of Knowledge Acquisition , 1995 .

[13]  D. Klahr “ To Every Thing There is a Season, and a Time to Every Purpose Under the Heavens” What about Direct Instruction? , 2009 .

[14]  Deanna Kuhn,et al.  Is Direct Instruction an Answer to the Right Question? , 2007 .

[15]  Milena K. Nigam,et al.  The Equivalence of Learning Paths in Early Science Instruction: Effects of Direct Instruction and Discovery Learning , 2022 .

[16]  Slava Kalyuga,et al.  Measuring Knowledge to Optimize Cognitive Load Factors During Instruction. , 2004 .

[17]  Slava Kalyuga,et al.  When problem solving is superior to studying worked examples. , 2001 .

[18]  Paul J. Germann,et al.  Identifying patterns and relationships among the responses of seventh‐grade students to the science process skill of designing experiments , 1996 .

[19]  D. Kuhn,et al.  Direct instruction vs. discovery: The long view , 2007 .

[20]  D. Klahr,et al.  Developing elementary science skills: Instructional effectiveness and path independence , 2008 .