Brain activation during a social attribution task in adolescents with moderate to severe traumatic brain injury

The ability to make accurate judgments about the mental states of others, sometimes referred to as theory of mind (ToM), is often impaired following traumatic brain injury (TBI), and this deficit may contribute to problems with interpersonal relationships. The present study used an animated social attribution task (SAT) with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to examine structures mediating ToM in adolescents with moderate to severe TBI. The study design also included a comparison group of matched, typically developing (TD) adolescents. The TD group exhibited activation within a number of areas that are thought to be relevant to ToM, including the medial prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex, fusiform gyrus, and posterior temporal and parietal areas. The TBI subjects had significant activation within many of these same areas, but their activation was generally more intense and excluded the medial prefrontal cortex. Exploratory regression analyses indicated a negative relation between ToM-related activation and measures of white matter integrity derived from diffusion tensor imaging, while there was also a positive relation between activation and lesion volume. These findings are consistent with alterations in the level and pattern of brain activation that may be due to the combined influence of diffuse axonal injury and focal lesions.

[1]  V. Stone,et al.  Social and emotional competence in traumatic brain injury: New and established assessment tools , 2011, Social neuroscience.

[2]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  fMRI item analysis in a theory of mind task , 2011, NeuroImage.

[3]  Naomi J. Aldrich,et al.  Perspective taking in children's narratives about jealousy. , 2011, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[4]  Deirdre Wilson,et al.  What's in a voice? Prosody as a test case for the Theory of Mind account of autism , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  A. Simion,et al.  Exploring theory of mind after severe traumatic brain injury , 2010, Cortex.

[6]  H. Levin,et al.  Emotion recognition following pediatric traumatic brain injury: Longitudinal analysis of emotional prosody and facial emotion recognition , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  A. Cantagallo,et al.  Theory of Mind in patients with ventromedial or dorsolateral prefrontal lesions following traumatic brain injury , 2010, Brain injury.

[8]  S. Channon,et al.  Mentalising and social problem-solving after brain injury , 2010, Neuropsychological rehabilitation.

[9]  Ana C. Vasquez,et al.  Diffusion Tensor Imaging of the Cingulum Bundle in Children After Traumatic Brain Injury , 2010, Developmental neuropsychology.

[10]  J. Leon-Carrion,et al.  Theory of mind deficits in patients with acquired brain injury: A quantitative review , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  H. Levin,et al.  Diffusion Tensor Imaging Analysis of Frontal Lobes in Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury , 2010, Journal of child neurology.

[12]  Hanzhang Lu,et al.  Brain activation while thinking about the self from another person's perspective after traumatic brain injury in adolescents. , 2010, Neuropsychology.

[13]  M. Dennis,et al.  Theory of mind depends on domain-general executive functions of working memory and cognitive inhibition in children with traumatic brain injury , 2009, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[14]  Mary R. Newsome,et al.  Effects of severity of traumatic brain injury and brain reserve on cognitive-control related brain activation. , 2009, Journal of neurotrauma.

[15]  Raul Coimbra,et al.  Integrated imaging approach with MEG and DTI to detect mild traumatic brain injury in military and civilian patients. , 2009, Journal of neurotrauma.

[16]  A. Bailey,et al.  Are there theory of mind regions in the brain? A review of the neuroimaging literature , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[17]  R. Asarnow,et al.  Neurocognitive outcomes and recovery after pediatric TBI: meta-analytic review of the literature. , 2009, Neuropsychology.

[18]  Erin D. Bigler,et al.  Diffusion tensor imaging of hemispheric asymmetries in the developing brain , 2009, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[19]  Harvey S. Levin,et al.  Diffusion Tensor Imaging in Relation to Cognitive and Functional Outcome of Traumatic Brain Injury in Children , 2008, The Journal of head trauma rehabilitation.

[20]  F. Hillary Neuroimaging of working memory dysfunction and the dilemma with brain reorganization hypotheses , 2008, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[21]  M. Mccarthy,et al.  Executive Functioning in the First Year After Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury , 2008, Pediatrics.

[22]  S. Blakemore The social brain in adolescence , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[23]  Aaron C. Koralek,et al.  Two Takes on the Social Brain: A Comparison of Theory of Mind Tasks , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[24]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Spatial normalization of lesioned brains: Performance evaluation and impact on fMRI analyses , 2007, NeuroImage.

[25]  T. Ohnishi,et al.  Changes of brain activity in the neural substrates for theory of mind during childhood and adolescence , 2007, Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[26]  Randy L. Gollub,et al.  Reproducibility of quantitative tractography methods applied to cerebral white matter , 2007, NeuroImage.

[27]  Keith Owen Yeates,et al.  Social outcomes in childhood brain disorder: a heuristic integration of social neuroscience and developmental psychology. , 2007, Psychological bulletin.

[28]  G. Glover,et al.  Children's and adults’ neural bases of verbal and nonverbal ‘theory of mind’ , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  J. Henry,et al.  Theory of mind following traumatic brain injury: The role of emotion recognition and executive dysfunction , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  Erin D Bigler,et al.  Diffusion tensor imaging in the corpus callosum in children after moderate to severe traumatic brain injury. , 2006, Journal of neurotrauma.

[31]  Heidi Johansen-Berg,et al.  Functional anatomy of interhemispheric cortical connections in the human brain , 2006, Journal of anatomy.

[32]  Lindsey J. Powell,et al.  It's the Thought That Counts , 2006, Psychological science.

[33]  S. Chapman,et al.  Predictors of personality change due to traumatic brain injury in children and adolescents six to twenty-four months after injury. , 2006, The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[34]  F. Knott,et al.  Theory of mind in children with traumatic brain injury , 2006, Brain injury.

[35]  D. Katz,et al.  Update of Neuropathology and Neurological Recovery After Traumatic Brain Injury , 2005, The Journal of head trauma rehabilitation.

[36]  Thomas Netsch,et al.  Quantitative evaluation of image-based distortion correction in diffusion tensor imaging , 2004, IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging.

[37]  U. Frith,et al.  The impact of extensive medial frontal lobe damage on 'Theory of Mind' and cognition. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[38]  Caterina Mainero,et al.  fMRI evidence of brain reorganization during attention and memory tasks in multiple sclerosis , 2004, NeuroImage.

[39]  Linda Ewing-Cobbs,et al.  Modeling of Longitudinal Academic Achievement Scores After Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury , 2004, Developmental neuropsychology.

[40]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  The detection of contingency and animacy from simple animations in the human brain. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[41]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[42]  P. Skudlarski,et al.  The role of the fusiform face area in social cognition: implications for the pathobiology of autism. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[43]  S. Isenberg Integrated imaging , 2002, BDJ.

[44]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Functional Imaging Studies of Neuropsychological Patients: Applications and Limitations , 2002, Neurocase.

[45]  H. Taylor,et al.  Social Problem-Solving Skills in Children with Traumatic Brain Injury: Long-Term Outcomes and Prediction of Social Competence , 2002, Child neuropsychology : a journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence.

[46]  C. Frith,et al.  Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[47]  M M Mesulam,et al.  Impact of signal-to-noise on functional MRI of the human amygdala , 2001, Neuroreport.

[48]  B M Bly,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging of working memory impairment after traumatic brain injury , 2001, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[49]  Maureen Dennis,et al.  Understanding of Literal Truth, Ironic Criticism, and Deceptive Praise Following Childhood Head Injury , 2001, Brain and Language.

[50]  J. DeLuca,et al.  Functionally Activated Brain Imaging (O‐15 PET and fMRI) in the Study of Learning and Memory after Traumatic Brain Injury , 2001, The Journal of head trauma rehabilitation.

[51]  R. Adolphs The neurobiology of social cognition , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[52]  D. M. Jacobs,et al.  Learning to perceive the relative mass of colliding balls: The effects of ratio scaling and feedback , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[53]  C. Frith,et al.  Movement and Mind: A Functional Imaging Study of Perception and Interpretation of Complex Intentional Movement Patterns , 2000, NeuroImage.

[54]  P. V. van Zijl,et al.  Three‐dimensional tracking of axonal projections in the brain by magnetic resonance imaging , 1999, Annals of neurology.

[55]  R. Knight,et al.  Frontal Lobe Contributions to Theory of Mind , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[56]  J. Fletcher,et al.  Concept formation and problem-solving following closed head injury in children , 1997, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[57]  R M Weisskoff,et al.  Simple measurement of scanner stability for functional NMR imaging of activation in the brain , 1996, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[58]  D. Bowler "Theory of mind" in Asperger's syndrome. , 1992, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[59]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[60]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind” ? , 1985, Cognition.

[61]  B. Jennett,et al.  Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness. A practical scale. , 1974, Lancet.

[62]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[63]  F. Heider,et al.  An experimental study of apparent behavior , 1944 .

[64]  Claire Hughes,et al.  Individual differences in false belief understanding are stable from 3 to 6 years of age and predict children's mental state talk with school friends. , 2011, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[65]  David A. Lishner,et al.  I'm Embarrassed for You: The Effect of Valuing and Perspective Taking on Empathic Embarrassment and Empathic Concern , 2011 .

[66]  4. Changing brains, changing perspectives: The neurocognitive development of reciprocity , 2011 .

[67]  L. Turkstra,et al.  Theory of Mind and social beliefs in adolescents with traumatic brain injury. , 2004, NeuroRehabilitation.

[68]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[69]  D. Doyle,et al.  Diffuse brain damage of immediate impact type. Its relationship to 'primary brain-stem damage' in head injury. , 1977, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[70]  R. C. Oldfield THE ASSESSMENT AND ANALYSIS OF HANDEDNESS , 1971 .