Thermosensory reversal effect quantified.

At room temperature, some materials feel colder than others due to differences in thermal conductivity, heat capacity and geometry. When the ambient temperature is well above skin temperature, the roles of 'cold' and 'warm' materials are reversed. In this paper, this effect is quantified by measuring discrimination thresholds for subjective coldness at different ambient temperatures using stimuli of different thicknesses. The reversal point was found to be at 34 degrees C, somewhat above skin temperature. At this reversal point, discrimination is quite impossible. At room temperature, subjects were able to discriminate between stimuli of different thickness based on subjective coldness, showing that the sense of touch, unlike vision, can penetrate solid objects. Furthermore, somewhat surprisingly, at ambient temperatures well below normal room temperature, discrimination is worse than at room temperature.

[1]  D. Kenshalo,et al.  Warm and cool thresholds as a function of rate of stimulus temperature change , 1968 .

[2]  J. Greenspan,et al.  Thermosensory intensity and affect throughout the perceptible range , 2003, Somatosensory & motor research.

[3]  K. Buettner,et al.  THERMAL CONTACT COEFFICIENT(A TERM PROPOSED FOR USE IN HEAT TRANSFER) , 1961 .

[4]  L.A. Jones,et al.  Material identification using real and simulated thermal cues , 2004, The 26th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society.

[5]  Lynette A. Jones,et al.  Material discrimination and thermal perception , 2003, 11th Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems, 2003. HAPTICS 2003. Proceedings..

[6]  K. Offord,et al.  Description of “Minnesota Thermal Disks” and normal values of cutaneous thermal discrimination in man , 1974, Neurology.

[7]  W. M. Bergmann Tiest,et al.  An Experimentally Verified Model of the Perceived 'Coldness' of Objects , 2007, WHC.

[8]  J. C. Stevens,et al.  Temperature sensitivity of the body surface over the life span. , 1998, Somatosensory & motor research.

[9]  M. Bushnell,et al.  Temporal and qualitative properties of cold pain and heat pain: a psychophysical study , 1998, Pain.

[10]  K. O. Johnson,et al.  Peripheral neural determinants of temperature discrimination in man: a correlative study of responses to cooling skin. , 1973, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  Hsin-Ni Ho,et al.  Contribution of thermal cues to material discrimination and localization , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.

[12]  Stanley Coren,et al.  The Left-Hander Syndrome: The Causes and Consequences of Left-Handedness , 1992 .

[13]  Akio Yamamoto,et al.  Control of thermal tactile display based on prediction of contact temperature , 2004, IEEE International Conference on Robotics and Automation, 2004. Proceedings. ICRA '04. 2004.

[14]  Darwin G. Caldwell,et al.  Enhanced tactile feedback (tele-taction) using a multi-functional sensory system , 1993, [1993] Proceedings IEEE International Conference on Robotics and Automation.

[15]  A. Kappers,et al.  Tactile perception of thermal diffusivity , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[16]  Shuichi Ino,et al.  A tactile display for presenting quality of materials by changing the temperature of skin surface , 1993, Proceedings of 1993 2nd IEEE International Workshop on Robot and Human Communication.