Alignment as the Basis for Successful Communication
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Rolf A. Zwaan,et al. Situation models in language comprehension and memory. , 1998, Psychological bulletin.
[2] Z. Harris,et al. Foundations of Language , 1940 .
[3] G. Altmann,et al. Incremental interpretation at verbs: restricting the domain of subsequent reference , 1999, Cognition.
[4] G. Seth. Psychology of Language , 1968, Nature.
[5] B. Webber,et al. Elements of Discourse Understanding , 1983 .
[6] M. Pickering,et al. The representation of lexical and syntactic information in bilinguals: Evidence from syntactic priming , 2007 .
[7] William S. Horton,et al. Conversational Common Ground and Memory Processes in Language Production , 2005 .
[8] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Speech listening specifically modulates the excitability of tongue muscles: a TMS study , 2002, The European journal of neuroscience.
[9] Philip R. Cohen,et al. Referring as a Collaborative Process , 2003 .
[10] K. A. Ericsson,et al. Long-term working memory. , 1995, Psychological review.
[11] Stephanie Kelter,et al. Surface form and memory in question answering , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.
[12] M. Pickering,et al. Why is conversation so easy? , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[13] Michael F. Schober,et al. Speakers, addressees, and frames of reference: Whose effort is minimized in conversations about locations? , 1995 .
[14] S. Gries. Syntactic Priming: A Corpus-based Approach , 2005, Journal of psycholinguistic research.
[15] Alexandra A. Cleland,et al. Syntactic co-ordination in dialogue , 2000, Cognition.
[16] J. Fodor,et al. The Psychology of Language , 1974 .
[17] H. H. Clark,et al. References in Conversation Between Experts and Novices , 1987 .
[18] T. Chartrand,et al. The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[19] Eve V. Clark,et al. The Lexicon in Acquisition , 1996 .
[20] B. Keysar,et al. When do speakers take into account common ground? , 1996, Cognition.
[21] M. Tomasello,et al. Young children learn to produce passives with nonce verbs. , 1999, Developmental psychology.
[22] A. Meltzoff,et al. Imitation of Facial and Manual Gestures by Human Neonates , 1977, Science.
[23] M. Banaji,et al. Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. , 1995, Psychological review.
[24] Anne Cutler,et al. Twenty-first century psycholinguistics : four cornerstones , 2005 .
[25] H. H. Clark,et al. Concealing one's meaning from overhearers , 1987 .
[26] R. Kirk. CONVENTION: A PHILOSOPHICAL STUDY , 1970 .
[27] B. Danet,et al. ’Baby’ or ’fetus’?: Language and the construction of reality in a manslaughter trial , 1980 .
[28] S. Levinson. Space in language and cognition: Explorations in cognitive diversity , 2003 .
[29] Ray Jackendoff,et al. Parallel constraint-based generative theories of language , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[30] S. Garrod,et al. Saying what you mean in dialogue: A study in conceptual and semantic co-ordination , 1987, Cognition.
[31] Simon Garrod,et al. Establishing and Using Routines During Dialogue: Implications for Psychology and Linguistics , 2005 .
[32] H. H. Clark,et al. Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[33] Sandra Truscott,et al. Saying What You Mean , 1998 .
[34] S. Garrod,et al. Conversation, co-ordination and convention: an empirical investigation of how groups establish linguistic conventions , 1994, Cognition.
[35] M. Pickering,et al. Alignment of reference frames in dialogue , 2004 .
[36] M. F. Schober. Spatial perspective-taking in conversation , 1993, Cognition.
[37] M. Pickering,et al. Is Syntax Separate or Shared Between Languages? , 2004, Psychological science.
[38] Z. Harris,et al. Foundations of language , 1941 .
[39] H. H. Clark,et al. Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation. , 1996 .
[40] Bernhard Hommel,et al. Codes and their vicissitudes , 2001 .
[41] Alexandra A. Cleland,et al. The use of lexical and syntactic information in language production: Evidence from the priming of noun-phrase structure , 2003 .
[42] M. Arbib,et al. Language within our grasp , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.
[43] G. Aschersleben,et al. The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.
[44] Karin Aijmer,et al. Conversational Routines in English: Convention and Creativity , 1996 .
[45] David Lewis. Convention: A Philosophical Study , 1986 .
[46] Michael K. Tanenhaus,et al. Parsing in a Dynamical System: An Attractor-based Account of the Interaction of Lexical and Structural Constraints in Sentence Processing , 1997 .
[47] M. Pickering,et al. Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[48] Benedikt Szmrecsanyi. Language users as creatures of habit: A corpus-based analysis of persistence in spoken English , 2005 .
[49] D. Tannen. Talking Voices: Repetition, Dialogue, and Imagery in Conversational Discourse , 1989 .
[50] H. H. Clark,et al. Referring as a collaborative process , 1986, Cognition.
[51] G. Button,et al. Talk and Social Organisation , 1988 .
[52] W. Levelt,et al. Speaking: From Intention to Articulation , 1990 .
[53] C. Fowler,et al. Rapid access to speech gestures in perception: Evidence from choice and simple response time tasks. , 2003, Journal of memory and language.
[54] Koenraad Kuiper. Smooth Talkers: The Linguistic Performance of Auctioneers and Sportscasters , 1995 .
[55] M. Pickering,et al. Do Speakers Avoid Ambiguities During Dialogue? , 2005, Psychological science.
[56] S. Goldinger. Echoes of echoes? An episodic theory of lexical access. , 1998, Psychological review.