Neither here nor there: localizing conflicting visual attributes.
暂无分享,去创建一个
David Whitaker | Paul V McGraw | David R Badcock | Jennifer Skillen | D. Whitaker | P. McGraw | D. Badcock | Jennifer Skillen
[1] M. J. Morgan,et al. Motion displacement thresholds for compound stimuli predicted by the displacement of centroids , 1994, Vision Research.
[2] J J Knierim,et al. Neural responses to visual texture patterns in middle temporal area of the macaque monkey. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.
[3] D. Levi,et al. The influence of adaptation on perceived visual location , 1997, Vision Research.
[4] S. Grossberg,et al. Neural dynamics of form perception: boundary completion, illusory figures, and neon color spreading. , 1985 .
[5] H. Wilson,et al. Fourier and Non-Fourier Pattern Discrimination Compared , 1996, Vision Research.
[6] D. Badcock,et al. Global-motion Perception: Interaction of Chromatic and Luminance Signals , 1996, Vision Research.
[7] J. C. Meadows,et al. Cerebral color blindness: An acquired defect in hue discrimination , 1979, Annals of neurology.
[8] R. J. Watt,et al. Mechanisms responsible for the assessment of visual location: Theory and evidence , 1983, Vision Research.
[9] I Mareschal,et al. Cortical processing of second-order motion , 1999, Visual Neuroscience.
[10] M. Landy,et al. Measurement and modeling of depth cue combination: in defense of weak fusion , 1995, Vision Research.
[11] D. Hubel,et al. Segregation of form, color, movement, and depth: anatomy, physiology, and perception. , 1988, Science.
[12] Andrew T. Smith,et al. Evidence for separate motion-detecting mechanisms for first- and second-order motion in human vision , 1994, Vision Research.
[13] John H. R. Maunsell,et al. Hierarchical organization and functional streams in the visual cortex , 1983, Trends in Neurosciences.
[14] George Mather,et al. Depth cue integration: stereopsis and image blur , 2000, Vision Research.
[15] M. Just,et al. Cognitive coordinate systems: accounts of mental rotation and individual differences in spatial ability. , 1985, Psychological review.
[16] DAVID WHITAKER,et al. Centroid Analysis Predicts Visual Localization of First- and Second-order Stimuli , 1996, Vision Research.
[17] H. Wilson,et al. A psychophysically motivated model for two-dimensional motion perception , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.
[18] Denis G. Pelli,et al. Accurate control of contrast on microcomputer displays , 1991, Vision Research.
[19] G. Sperling,et al. Drift-balanced random stimuli: a general basis for studying non-Fourier motion perception. , 1988, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[20] M. Morgan,et al. Efficiency of locating centres of dot-clusters by human observers , 1991, Vision Research.
[21] Isabelle Mareschal,et al. A cortical locus for the processing of contrast-defined contours , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.
[22] A. Derrington,et al. Discriminating the direction of second-order motion at short stimulus durations , 1993, Vision Research.
[23] D R Badcock,et al. Independent first- and second-order motion energy analyses of optic flow , 2001, Psychological research.
[24] Patrick Cavanagh,et al. Interattribute apparent motion , 1989, Vision Research.
[25] A. Dale,et al. From retinotopy to recognition: fMRI in human visual cortex , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[26] J. Movshon,et al. Spatial summation in the receptive fields of simple cells in the cat's striate cortex. , 1978, The Journal of physiology.
[27] D. Hubel,et al. Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex , 1962, The Journal of physiology.
[28] Josée Rivest,et al. Localizing contours defined by more than one attribute , 1996, Vision Research.
[29] David R. Badcock,et al. Detecting the displacement of periodic patterns , 1985, Vision Research.
[30] D. V. van Essen,et al. Antibody labeling of functional subdivisions in visual cortex: Cat-301 immunoreactivity in striate and extrastriate cortex of the macaque monkey , 1990, Visual Neuroscience.
[31] Norma Graham,et al. Nonlinear processes in spatial-frequency channel models of perceived texture segregation: Effects of sign and amount of contrast , 1992, Vision Research.
[32] S. Shipp,et al. The functional logic of cortical connections , 1988, Nature.
[33] Suzanne P. McKee,et al. Integration regions for visual hyperacuity , 1977, Vision Research.
[34] M J Morgan,et al. Vernier acuity predicted from changes in the light distribution of the retinal image. , 1985, Spatial vision.
[35] M. Morgan,et al. What direction of motion do we see if luminance but not colour contrast is reversed during displacement? psychophysical evidence for a signed-colour input to motion detection , 1994, Vision Research.
[36] D. Regan,et al. Vernier step acuity and bisection acuity for texture-defined form , 1997, Vision Research.
[37] M. J. Morgan,et al. Stereo and motion disparities interfere with positional averaging , 1993, Vision Research.
[38] A. Damasio,et al. Central achromatopsia , 1980, Neurology.
[39] David Whitaker,et al. Spatial characteristics of the second-order visual pathway revealed by positional adaptation , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[40] R. Shapley. Linearity and non-linearity in cortical receptive fields. , 1994, Ciba Foundation symposium.
[41] N. Mai,et al. Selective disturbance of movement vision after bilateral brain damage. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[42] M. J. Morgan,et al. Positional acuity with chromatic stimuli , 1985, Vision Research.
[43] C L Baker,et al. A processing stream in mammalian visual cortex neurons for non-Fourier responses. , 1993, Science.
[44] M S Landy,et al. Ideal cue combination for localizing texture-defined edges. , 2001, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.